Free Standard AU & NZ Shipping For All Book Orders Over $80!
Register      Login
Journal of Primary Health Care Journal of Primary Health Care Society
Journal of The Royal New Zealand College of General Practitioners
RESEARCH ARTICLE (Open Access)

Agreement between parental perception of child weight status and actual weight status is similar across different ethnic groups in New Zealand

Kim Meredith-Jones 1 , Sheila Williams 2 , Rachael Taylor 1
+ Author Affiliations
- Author Affiliations

1 Department of Medicine, University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand

2 Department of Preventive and Social Medicine, University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand

3 Correspondence to: Kim Meredith-Jones Department of Medicine, University of Otago, PO Box 56 Dunedin, New Zealand kim.meredith-jones@otago.ac.nz

Journal of Primary Health Care 8(4) 316-324 https://doi.org/10.1071/HC16002
Published: 5 September 2016

Journal Compilation © Royal New Zealand College of General Practitioners 2016.
This is an open access article licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License.

Abstract

INTRODUCTION: Accurate parental perception of their child's weight is poor. Accuracy may be influenced by differences in ethnicity but this is currently unknown.

AIM: To determine whether agreement between parental perception of child weight status and actual child weight status differs according to ethnic group (NZ European, Māori, Pacific, Asian), and to investigate whether it is influenced by various demographic and behavioural factors.

METHODS: A total of 1093 children (4–8 years old) attended a weight screening initiative. Parents completed questionnaires on demographics, beliefs about child weight, parenting style, parental feeding practices and social desirability. Actual measured weight status was compared with parental perception of weight status (underweight, normal weight, overweight).

RESULTS: Agreement about child weight status was apparent in 85% of NZ European, 84% of Māori, 82% of Pacific and 88% of Asian children. However, adjusting for chance led to kappas of 0.34, 0.38, 0.41 and 0.53, respectively, indicating only fair-to-moderate agreement. Overall, agreement between measured body mass index and parental perception was not related to ethnic group, child sex and age, maternal age and education, and household deprivation (k ranged from 0.16 to 0.47). However, agreement about weight status was higher in parents who reported higher levels of restrictive feeding than in parents who reported less restriction (P < 0.01) but agreement was only fair.

CONCLUSION: Agreement between parental perception and actual weight status was fair and did not differ between the ethnic groups examined.

KEYWORDS: Ethnicity; parental perception; child; overweight; BMI

WHAT GAP THIS FILLS
What is already known: Parental perception of overweight in their child is poor and a limited number of studies suggest that ethnic differences in parental perception exist.
What this study adds: Parental perception of their child’s weight does not appear to differ across ethnic groups in New Zealand and is not related to behavioural or demographic differences.



Introduction

In the past 30 years, the prevalence of overweight and obesity has rapidly increased worldwide, with more than 20% of boys and girls now considered to be overweight or obese.1 Obesity rates are similar in New Zealand, and vary with ethnicity, with overweight and obesity rates as high as 45% and 55% in Māori and Pacific children, respectively.2

Despite this widespread phenomenon, there is overwhelming evidence that the majority of parents with overweight children fail to recognise their child as such.3,4 Several characteristics are thought to influence parental recognition of overweight in their children, including age, sex and weight status of the child,510 as well as weight and educational level of the parents, and family income.6,7 A limited number of studies internationally suggest that ethnic differences in parental perception may be apparent because larger body sizes are more tolerated in certain ethnic groups.1113 However, although parental underestimation of child weight appears common among all parents, regardless of ethnic group, whether accuracy in perception varies among different ethnic groups in New Zealand is unknown.

Given the lack of comparable research in New Zealand Māori, and the clear disparities in weight status and weight-related comorbidities between ethnic minorities and New Zealand Europeans,2 the aims of our study were to: (1) determine whether the level of agreement in parental perception of child weight status and children’s actual weight status differed according to child ethnic group; and (2) examine other factors that might underlie weight misperception within an ethnically diverse population in New Zealand.


Methods

This analysis involved Phase 1 of our Motivational Interviewing and Treatment (MInT) study, which investigated different methods for informing parents that their young child was overweight or obese after a weight screening initiative. Ethical approval was obtained from the Lower South Regional Ethics Committee (LRS/09/09/039) and all parents gave informed consent.

The study has previously been described in detail.14 In brief, all families with children aged 4–8 years enrolled at nine participating general practices or attending secondary care clinics were invited to participate in a comprehensive health check. Children were excluded if they had severe childhood arthritis, severe asthma, cystic fibrosis, inflammatory bowel disease, congenital or chromosomal abnormalities, severe developmental delay, were on medication that may influence body composition or their families were not planning to remain in the region for the next two years.

At the health check session, duplicate measures of height (Tanita portable stadiometer) and weight (Tanita BC-418) were obtained following standard techniques, and body mass index (BMI) was calculated. BMI z-scores were obtained using USA reference data,15 and body composition was assessed using bioelectrical impedance (Tanita BC-418). Excess body fat was defined using the age- and sex-specific body fat criteria, described by McCarthy et al.16 While children were being measured, their main caregiver (virtually all mothers) completed a comprehensive online questionnaire assessing demographic characteristics including child ethnic group, maternal education and maternal age. Child ethnicity was categorised as New Zealand European and Others (NZEO), Māori, Pacific or Asian. The ‘Other’ ethnic group (comprising mainly Middle Eastern, Latin American and African ethnic groups) has been combined with ‘European’ due to small numbers. An index of socioeconomic status (New Zealand deprivation index, NZDep200617) was obtained from their residential address. Parents were also asked to complete questionnaires on parenting style18 and feeding practices,19 as described below. The Comprehensive Feeding Practices questionnaire has 12 subscales including monitoring, environment, use of food as a reward, restriction for weight and restriction for health. Parental perception of their child’s weight status was assessed using a five-point Likert scale question (where 1 = underweight; 2 = a little underweight; 3 = about right; 4 = a little overweight; 5 = overweight for levels of perception). Scores of 1 and 2 were combined to describe underweight and scores of 4 and 5 were combined to describe overweight for final analyses. Parent scores were then compared to the child’s actual BMI classification where underweight ≤ 3rd centile; normal weight = 3rd–84th centile; overweight and obese ≥ 85th centile. Social desirability was assessed using the 13-item short form of the Marlowe–Crowne Social Desirability scale (MCSDS), where higher scores indicate more generally socially desirable responses.20 Maternal BMI was obtained for 97% of mothers (3% missing), with 49% from duplicate measurements in the clinic and 48% from self-reported data. Once the questionnaire had been completed, parents were informed about the weight status of their child.21

Statistical analyses

Descriptive statistics were used to summarise the characteristics of the sample. Cross-classification was used to show the correspondence between perceived weight status (the proxy measure) and actual weight status (the gold standard), classified as underweight, normal weight and obese for each ethnic group. As the interpretation of agreement between a proxy measure and a gold standard depends on chance, the kappa statistic is used to assess agreement.22 As ethnicity was our primary focus, we calculated a weighted kappa for the whole sample and separately for each ethnic group. Because we were also interested in several other variables, we used two comprehensive ordinal logistic regression models, with a cumulative logistic link, to estimate the marginal probabilities of obtaining the three outcomes in the proxy and gold standard measure. These were used to estimate chance agreement, which was then used to adjust agreement to obtain Cohen’s kappa. Linear regression was used to obtain estimates of kappa for several variables of interest. The method is described in detail by Lipsitz et al.23 Kappa statistics measure the level of agreement between two measurements beyond what would be expected by chance. Kappa values were interpreted as follows: k < 0.00 = poor agreement; k between 0.20 and 0.40 = fair agreement; k between 0.40 and 0.60 = moderate agreement; k between 0.61 and 0.80 = good agreement; and k ≥ 0.80 = very good agreement.22


Results

A total of 1093 children attended the health check session (Table 1). Similar to current New Zealand population data,24 15% of children were classified as overweight (BMI ≥ 85th < 95th), with an additional 10% being classified as obese (BMI ≥ 95th).


Table 1. Characteristics of the study population
Click to zoom

Agreement between the parental perception of weight status and actual weight status was 85% for the whole sample. As the chance agreement was 76%, kappa was 0.35 (95% CI: 0.32, 0.39), showing that agreement was only fair. The percentage agreement and kappa values were 85% and 0.34 (CI: 0.29, 0.38) for NZEO, 84% and 0.38 (CI: 0.28, 0.48) for Māori, 82% and 0.41 (CI: 0.20, 0.62) for Pacific and 88% and 0.53 (CI: 0.35, 0.72) for Asian children.

While differences between ethnic groups in the proportion of parents correctly identifying their child as being overweight were not apparent (Table 2), mean BMI z-scores (data not shown) were significantly higher in Māori and Pacific children (P < 0.01) compared to NZ European and Asian children, indicating that Māori and Pacific children may be significantly more overweight before parents accurately identify the problem. Misclassification of weight status using percentage fat cut-offs showed similar results, with more than half of parents of children with body fat percentages above the 85th percentile rating their child as normal weight (Table 3). Percentage agreement and kappa for fat percentage were 67% and 0.35 (CI: 0.30, 0.40) for the whole sample, 67% and 0.34 (CI: 0.28, 0.39) for NZEO, 66% and 0.36 (CI: 0.23, 0.49) for Māori, 78% and 0.56 (CI: 0.31, 0.80) for Pacific and 60% and 0.20 (CI: –0.01, 0.42, P = 0.04) for Asian children.


Table 2. Misclassification of weight status by ethnic group
Click to zoom


Table 3. Misclassification of weight status according to % fat cut-offs by ethnic group
Click to zoom

Table 4 reports the level of agreement between parental perception and actual weight status for a variety of demographic and behavioural measures. The kappa values in the first column indicate the level of agreement for each of the variables of interest. Although the agreement between perception and actual weight is higher than that expected by chance, the kappa values of 0.16 to 0.47 indicate poor to fair agreement. After adjusting for sex, ethnic group, household structure, maternal age and BMI, deprivation score, child health, concern about weight and ratings of physical activity, the overall chance agreement increased to 58% and the magnitude kappas estimated from the regression model were similar (Table 4).


Table 4. Strength of agreement and linear regression estimates between perception of child weight status by parent and measured body mass index (BMI) categories based on Cohen’s kappa
Click to zoom

Differences in kappa between different categories of a particular variable are shown in the second column. For example, while agreement between perceived and actual weight status was apparent for each individual ethnic group, there were no differences in the level of agreement, above that expected by chance, between the ethnic groups. Similarly, differences in the other measured behavioural and demographic variables did not influence the level of agreement. The single exception observed was for restrictive feeding practices; the level of agreement was higher for parents in the highest quarter of restrictive feeding, with an increase in kappa of 0.24 compared with the lowest quarter (P = 0.02). However, this finding should be interpreted with caution as agreement is poor in both cases and a large number of tests were undertaken.


Discussion

Consistent with international findings, parents of New Zealand children do not recognise excess weight in their overweight child. Importantly, this lack of agreement does not differ by ethnic group, with parents of children from all four groups studied reporting similar low levels of agreement. Level of agreement in parental perception is also not associated with any other demographic variable tested.

Despite similar levels of agreement among parents, the BMI z-scores among Māori and Pacific children were significantly greater compared to their Asian and NZEO counterparts. Qualitative studies have shown that Māori and Pacific families do not believe the bodyweight of their child is an issue until there are signs that it is directly affecting the child concerned, either physically or mentally.25 Given the high rates of obesity among Māori and Pacific families, heavier children may be more prevalent, making visual comparison with peers an unreliable indicator of overweight status for parents. Although some international studies have reported that Latino/Hispanic26 and African-American parents13 prefer large body types and do not identify their child’s excess weight as a problem, recent reviews suggest that parental underestimates of overweight/obese status is a common phenomenon among parents of overweight/obese children, regardless of the child’s ethnic group, gender or the parent’s age or own weight status.4,27

From a clinical perspective, recognition of overweight before the child is above the 95th centile is important.28 Inaccurate perception of overweight at lower BMI z-scores means that Māori and Pacific parents may not seek treatment for their child until they are considerably more overweight. In turn, this conceivably limits introducing smaller, more achievable behavioural changes and means larger changes are required to make a difference to weight; posing a greater challenge. Given the high risk for adult obesity and obesity-related conditions among Māori and Pacific people, and the high rates of overweight in their children,24 improving accuracy in identification at earlier stages of overweight is warranted.

While several studies have identified that parental perception of their children’s weight status is poor, the reasons for this misperception are not clear. Some suggest mothers may put greater emphasis on other factors such as physical activity and social participation,29 or may be better at identifying excess weight in older rather than younger children.5 In a large, representative sample of children aged 5–12 and 13–17 years in Ireland, Hudson et al.7 reported that inaccurate perception of overweight was higher in younger children and suggested that lack of awareness may be particularly evident for younger children because the prevailing health message to mothers of young children is to encourage growth and keep pace with centile charts. For comparison, we calculated the kappa coefficient on the whole sample, categorising the groups into normal weight and overweight and obese (2×2 table) from the results reported by Hudson et al.7 Similar to our results, the level of agreement was fair in this sample (k = 0.30); therefore, it is feasible that the higher levels of agreement at older ages reported by Hudson et al.7 may have been due to chance. However, given that other studies6,30 have shown parents are better at correctly identifying overweight in older versus younger children and the relatively restricted age range in our study, further research is required.

Unlike previous studies that have reported associations between parental misclassification and factors such as sex of the child,31 low maternal education level,32 lower socioeconomic status7 and higher maternal BMI,33 our study found no relationship with these variables. However, none of the previous research has adjusted for chance agreement (such as by using Cohen’s kappa), which could lead to inflated estimates and false conclusions. In our study, those who reported the highest level of restrictive feeding practices were significantly more accurate at perceiving overweight in their child. Several recent studies have also demonstrated an association between restrictive feeding and accurate perception of their child’s weight.3436 Although cross-sectional studies such as ours cannot demonstrate causality, previous studies have also reported that parents use restrictive feeding practices to a greater extent when they are concerned about children’s weight.36 Although earlier work thought that higher maternal restriction increased the risk of future weight gain37,38 or eating in the absence of hunger,38 more recent work has shown that higher degrees of maternal restriction are associated with decreased BMI in longitudinal analyses.39,40 Given these conflicting data, it is important for health professionals to understand that parents who have high levels of concern for their child’s weight may use restrictive feeding practices that could have unintended consequences.36

The strengths of our study include the use of Cohen’s kappa to control for chance agreement and our recruitment strategy. We also included objective measures of height and weight. Because some debate remains over the comparability of BMI as a measure of body composition in children from different ethnic groups,41,42 we were also able to show that parental inaccuracy in perception is not because BMI does not distinguish between body composition differences in children from different ethnic groups. This observation supports earlier work from our group, showing that parental perception is poor even when accurate estimates of body composition are obtained by dual-energy x-ray absorptiometry.3 Our study also has some limitations. Although more than half of mothers’ heights and weights were directly measured, 48% were self-report and therefore maternal BMI may have been underestimated. Education levels were high, with 40% of mothers having a university degree, although a wide range of household deprivation was still observed. Our participants were mainly European, with underrepresentation of Pacific Island (8%) and Asian (12%) children.43 However, our numbers of Māori children reflected national figures (15%) and were higher than is typically observed locally (8%).43 No formal power analysis was performed for the outcomes of interest. Instead, the statistical power is reflected in the widths of the reported confidence intervals. As the upper limits for the confidence intervals generated around the overall and subgroup kappas only surpassed 0.6 (our criterion for acceptable agreement, indicating ‘moderate agreement’) in two subgroups (Pacific and Asian, which were also the smallest subgroups), we can be confident in our conclusions that agreement between perceived and actual child weight status in our study was below acceptable levels overall and for all other subgroups (European and Māori). So, for this purpose, the secondary analysis was sufficiently powered, aside from those subgroups (Pacific and Asian). However, the relatively wide confidence intervals for differences in kappa between subgroups did not allow us to rule out potentially interesting differences in agreement between all pairs of subgroups.

In conclusion, despite the lack of evidence that ethnic group plays a role in parental misperceptions, the consequences of such misperception may be greater for Māori and Pacific children. This observation emphasises the importance of clinicians discussing the weight status of Māori and Pacific children with their parents as early as possible. However, given that many of the behavioural and demographic variables were unrelated to accuracy of parental perception of their child’s weight status, clinicians should inform all parents, regardless of ethnic group, education or household deprivation, of their child’s weight status and provide appropriate information to reduce the risk of their child becoming an overweight adult. Improved understanding of parental perception may improve how the risks of overweight are communicated to all parents and can increase engagement with behavioural interventions.21 It may be important for clinicians to reframe discussions of weight in terms of concern for future overweight as opposed to identifying current overweight, which may avoid the stigma of parental blame and initiate behaviour change.


FINANCIAL DISCLOSURE

The authors have indicated they have no financial relationships relevant to this article to disclose.


CONFLICT OF INTEREST

The authors have indicated they have no potential conflicts of interest to disclose.



ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

Funding provided by the Health Research Council of New Zealand. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.


References

[1]  Ng M, Fleming T, Robinson M, et al. Global, regional, and national prevalence of overweight and obesity in children and adults during 1980–2013: a systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2013. Lancet 2014; 384 766–81.
Global, regional, and national prevalence of overweight and obesity in children and adults during 1980–2013: a systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2013.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 24880830PubMed |

[2]  Ministry of Health. Annual update of key results 2013/2014. New Zealand Health Survey. Wellington: Ministry of Health; 2014.

[3]  Miller JC, Grant AM, Drummond BF, et al. DXA measurements confirm that parental perceptions of elevated adiposity in young children are poor. Obesity 2007; 15 165–71.
DXA measurements confirm that parental perceptions of elevated adiposity in young children are poor.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 17228044PubMed |

[4]  Lundahl A, Kidwell KM, Nelson TD. Parental underestimates of child weight: a meta-analysis. Pediatrics 2014; 133 e689–703.
Parental underestimates of child weight: a meta-analysis.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 24488736PubMed |

[5]  Carnell S, Edwards C, Croker H, et al. Parental perceptions of overweight in 3–5 y olds. Int J Obes. 2005; 29 353–5.
Parental perceptions of overweight in 3–5 y olds.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 1:STN:280:DC%2BD2M7jsFKgtg%3D%3D&md5=ae33a5edc563d4bbae457a1c9872eedfCAS |

[6]  Huang JS, Becerra K, Oda T, et al. Parental ability to discriminate the weight status of children: results of a survey. Pediatrics 2007; 120 e112–9.
Parental ability to discriminate the weight status of children: results of a survey.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 17606538PubMed |

[7]  Hudson E, McGloin A, McConnon A. Parental weight (mis)perceptions: factors influencing parents’ ability to correctly categorise their child’s weight status. Matern Child Health J. 2012; 16 1801–9.
Parental weight (mis)perceptions: factors influencing parents’ ability to correctly categorise their child’s weight status.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 22139047PubMed |

[8]  Moore LC, Harris CV, Bradlyn AS. Exploring the relationship between parental concern and the management of childhood obesity. Matern Child Health J. 2012; 16 902–8.
Exploring the relationship between parental concern and the management of childhood obesity.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 21594667PubMed |

[9]  Park MH, Falconer CL, Saxena S, et al. Perceptions of health risk among parents of overweight children: a cross-sectional study within a cohort. Prev Med. 2013; 57 55–9.
Perceptions of health risk among parents of overweight children: a cross-sectional study within a cohort.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 23591440PubMed |

[10]  Petricevic N, Puharic Z, Posavec M, et al. Family history and parental recognition of overweight in Croatian children. Eur J Pediatr. 2012; 171 1209–14.
Family history and parental recognition of overweight in Croatian children.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 22415408PubMed |

[11]  Adams AK, Quinn RA, Prince RJ. Low recognition of childhood overweight and disease risk among Native-American caregivers. Obes Res. 2005; 13 146–52.
Low recognition of childhood overweight and disease risk among Native-American caregivers.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 15761174PubMed |

[12]  Myers S, Vargas Z. Parental perceptions of the preschool obese child. Pediatr Nurs. 2000; 26 23–30.
| 1:STN:280:DC%2BD383otFCisw%3D%3D&md5=cba689f29dc2cf37181539298d743b93CAS | 12026313PubMed |

[13]  Young-Hyman D, Herman LJ, Scott DL, Schlundt DG. Care giver perception of children’s obesity-related health risk: a study of African American families. Obes Res. 2000; 8 241–8.
Care giver perception of children’s obesity-related health risk: a study of African American families.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 1:STN:280:DC%2BD3cvjtlyntg%3D%3D&md5=38bc0806fdedd22cb79a32ad541429ebCAS | 10832767PubMed |

[14]  Taylor RW, Brown D, Dawson AM, et al. Motivational interviewing for screening and feedback and encouraging lifestyle changes to reduce relative weight in 4–8 year old children: design of the MInT study. BMC Public Health. 2010; 10 271–82.
Motivational interviewing for screening and feedback and encouraging lifestyle changes to reduce relative weight in 4–8 year old children: design of the MInT study.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 20497522PubMed |

[15]  Kuczmarski RJ, Ogden CL, Guo SS, et al. 2000 CDC Growth Charts: United States. Adv Data. 2000; 8 190–27.

[16]  McCarthy HD, Cole TJ, Fry T, et al. Body fat reference curves for children. Int J Obes. 2006; 30 598–602.
Body fat reference curves for children.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 1:STN:280:DC%2BD287nsFymug%3D%3D&md5=4bade0640e18ea7c7b2c8b9cf5eccee7CAS |

[17]  White P, Gunston J, Salmon C, et al. Atlas of socioeconomic deprivation in New Zealand NZDep2006. Wellington, New Zealand: Ministry of Health; 2008.

[18]  Arnold DS, O’Leary SG, Wolff LS, Acker MM. The Parenting Scale: a measure of dysfunctional parenting in discipline situations. Psychol Assess. 1993; 5 137–44.
The Parenting Scale: a measure of dysfunctional parenting in discipline situations.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

[19]  Haszard JJ, Williams SM, Dawson AM, et al. Factor analysis of the comprehensive feeding practices questionnaire in a large sample of children. Appetite 2013; 62 110–8.
Factor analysis of the comprehensive feeding practices questionnaire in a large sample of children.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 23207187PubMed |

[20]  Reynolds W. Development of reliable and valid short forms of the Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability Scale. J Clin Psychol. 1982; 38 119–25.
Development of reliable and valid short forms of the Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability Scale.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

[21]  Dawson AM, Brown DA, Cox A, et al. Using motivational interviewing for weight feedback to parents of young children. J Paediatr Child Health. 2014; 50 461–70.
Using motivational interviewing for weight feedback to parents of young children.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 24617494PubMed |

[22]  Landis JR, Koch GG. The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data. Biometrics 1977; 33 159–74.
The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 1:STN:280:DyaE2s7jsFWqtA%3D%3D&md5=d04b91d0e72a7a4792cb73b7a1fd2cadCAS | 843571PubMed |

[23]  Lipsitz S, Parzen M, Fitzmaurice G, Klar N. A two-stage logistic regression model for analyzing inter-rater agreement. Psychometrika 2003; 68 289–98.
A two-stage logistic regression model for analyzing inter-rater agreement.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

[24]  Taylor RW, Williams SM, Dawson AM, et al. What factors influence uptake into family-based obesity treatment after weight screening? J Pediatr. 2013; 163 1657–62.e1.
What factors influence uptake into family-based obesity treatment after weight screening?Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 24075623PubMed |

[25]  Controller and Auditor-General. Evolving approach to combating child obesity. Wellington, New Zealand: Controller and Auditor-General; 2013.

[26]  Contento IR, Basch C, Zybert P. Body image, weight, and food choices of Latina women and their young children. J Nutr Educ Behav. 2003; 35 236–48.
Body image, weight, and food choices of Latina women and their young children.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 14521823PubMed |

[27]  Towns N, D’Auria J. Parental perceptions of their child’s overweight: an integrative review of the literature. J Pediatr Nurs. 2009; 24 115–30.
Parental perceptions of their child’s overweight: an integrative review of the literature.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 19268233PubMed |

[28]  Reilly JJ, Methven E, McDowell ZC, et al. Health consequences of obesity. Arch Dis Child. 2003; 88 748–52.
Health consequences of obesity.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 1:STN:280:DC%2BD3szpvFOjtw%3D%3D&md5=c9a768bf73f1b6334446a9882033707dCAS | 12937090PubMed |

[29]  Jain A, Sherman SN, Chamberlin DAL, et al. Why don’t low-income mothers worry about their preschoolers being overweight? Pediatrics 2001; 107 1138–46.
Why don’t low-income mothers worry about their preschoolers being overweight?Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 1:STN:280:DC%2BD3M3ptVCnsA%3D%3D&md5=8f15df4e30d01a998d2813863f356123CAS | 11331699PubMed |

[30]  Eckstein KC, Mikhail LM, Ariza AJ, et al. Parents’ perceptions of their child’s weight and health. Pediatrics 2006; 117 681–90.
| 16510647PubMed |

[31]  De La O A, Jordan KC, Ortiz K, et al. Do parents accurately perceive their child’s weight status? J Pediatr Health Care 2009; 23 216–21.
Do parents accurately perceive their child’s weight status?Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 19559989PubMed |

[32]  Baughcum AE, Chamberlin LA, Deeks CM, et al. Maternal perceptions of overweight preschool children. Pediatrics 2000; 106 1380–6.
Maternal perceptions of overweight preschool children.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 1:STN:280:DC%2BD3M%2FltVOrsg%3D%3D&md5=685c5342d422affd9f251007031e6cdeCAS | 11099592PubMed |

[33]  Chaparro MP, Langellier BA, Kim LP, Whaley SE. Predictors of accurate maternal perception of their preschool child’s weight status among Hispanic WIC participants. Obesity 2011; 19 2026–30.
Predictors of accurate maternal perception of their preschool child’s weight status among Hispanic WIC participants.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 21546937PubMed |

[34]  Gray WN, Janicke DM, Wistedt KM, Dumont-Driscoll MC. Factors associated with parental use of restrictive feeding practices to control their children’s food intake. Appetite 2010; 55 332–7.
Factors associated with parental use of restrictive feeding practices to control their children’s food intake.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 20633586PubMed |

[35]  Yilmaz R, Erkorkmaz U, Ozcetin M, Karaaslan E. How does parents’ visual perception of their child’s weight status affect their feeding style? Nutr Hosp 2013; 28 741–6.
| 23848098PubMed |

[36]  Keller KL, Olsen A, Kuilema L, et al. Predictors of parental perceptions and concerns about child weight. Appetite 2013; 62 96–102.
Predictors of parental perceptions and concerns about child weight.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 23207190PubMed |

[37]  Faith MS, Berkowitz RI, Stallings VA, et al. Parental feeding attitudes and styles and child body mass index: prospective analysis of a gene-environment interaction. Pediatrics 2004; 114 e429–36.
Parental feeding attitudes and styles and child body mass index: prospective analysis of a gene-environment interaction.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 15466068PubMed |

[38]  Birch LL, Fisher JO, Davison KK. Learning to overeat: maternal use of restrictive feeding practices promotes girls’ eating in the absence of hunger. Am J Clin Nutr. 2003; 78 215–20.
| 1:CAS:528:DC%2BD3sXlvF2isrY%3D&md5=4bebe2bd03eafbcc54e13c593f62fa88CAS | 12885700PubMed |

[39]  Campbell K, Andrianopoulos N, Hesketh K, et al. Parental use of restrictive feeding practices and child BMI z-score. A 3-year prospective cohort study. Appetite 2010; 55 84–8.
Parental use of restrictive feeding practices and child BMI z-score. A 3-year prospective cohort study.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 20420869PubMed |

[40]  Farrow CV, Blissett J. Controlling feeding practices: cause or consequence of early child weight? Pediatrics 2008; 121 e164–9.
| 18166535PubMed |

[41]  Swinburn BA, Ley SJ, Carmichael HE, Plank LD. Body size and composition in Polynesians. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord. 1999; 23 1178–83.
Body size and composition in Polynesians.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 1:STN:280:DC%2BD3c%2Fkslaluw%3D%3D&md5=bdde84fcd9d0fdec950157bcc31de77aCAS | 10578208PubMed |

[42]  Taylor RW, Brooking L, Williams SM, et al. Body mass index and waist circumference cutoffs to define obesity in indigenous New Zealanders. Am J Clin Nutr 2010; 92 390–7.
Body mass index and waist circumference cutoffs to define obesity in indigenous New Zealanders.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 1:CAS:528:DC%2BC3cXhtFWmsrbP&md5=f7f5b6d3998bd02fe163d111ca77da2eCAS | 20504973PubMed |

[43]  Statistics New Zealand. New Zealand in profile: 2015. Wellington, New Zealand; Statistics New Zealand; 2015.