Register      Login
Pacific Conservation Biology Pacific Conservation Biology Society
A journal dedicated to conservation and wildlife management in the Pacific region.
REVIEW

Utilising aversive conditioning to manage the behaviour of K’gari (Fraser Island) dingoes (Canis dingo)

Rob Appleby A C , Bradley Smith B , Lilia Bernede A and Darryl Jones A
+ Author Affiliations
- Author Affiliations

A Environmental Futures Research Institute, Griffith School of Environment, Griffith University, Nathan, Qld 4111, Australia.

B Appleton Institute, School of Human Health and Social Sciences, Central Queensland University, 44 Greenhill Road, Wayville, SA 5034, Australia.

C Corresponding author. Email: rob.appleby@wildspy.com.au

Pacific Conservation Biology 23(4) 335-358 https://doi.org/10.1071/PC17017
Submitted: 2 June 2017  Accepted: 23 October 2017   Published: 23 November 2017

Abstract

K’gari (Fraser Island) offers a rare opportunity for people to observe and encounter wild dingoes. Occasionally, however, such encounters can entail dingoes acting in a threatening or aggressive manner towards people, resulting in human injury and, in one tragic case, death. A suite of approaches aimed at minimising the risk to human safety posed by dingoes have been implemented on the island, including fencing, island-wide warning signage, and regulations against feeding. Despite such measures, negative encounters continue, and in cases where dingoes are deemed to pose an unacceptable risk, they are usually destroyed. In searching for non-lethal management alternatives, attempts have been made to modify undesirable dingo behaviour through aversive conditioning, but results to date have either been mixed or largely disappointing. Here we review a wide array of research that has utilised aversive stimuli in an effort to modify and manage the behaviour of wild animals, with a particular focus on related predators such as coyotes and wolves. We identified eight major categories of experimental research: conditioned taste aversion/avoidance (CTA), electric fencing, fladry, chemical repellents, fear-evoking stimuli, physical repellents, aversive collars/devices and hard release procedures. We then outline each of these categories in more detail, complete with pertinent examples of successes and failures as well as advantages and disadvantages. We conclude that some approaches offer promise within three main areas of incident mitigation experimentation: dingo exclusion (e.g. electric fencing), personal protection (mild chemical irritant sprays, sturdy umbrellas) and remedial aversive conditioning (e.g. shock collars). Other approaches, such as CTA and sublethal projectiles are not recommended. Like any approach, aversive conditioning is not a panacea, but it does offer promise in filling gaps in current management and as an alternative to lethal control.

Additional keywords: human–wildlife interaction, non-lethal management

Introduction

Certain species of carnivores can, under some circumstances, pose a threat to human safety, including the three species of North American bear (Ursus spp.) (Herrero 1970, 2002; Herrero and Fleck 1990; Herrero and Higgins 1999, 2003; Herrero et al. 2011), grey wolves (Canis lupus) (Linnell et al. 2002; McNay 2002), coyotes (Canis latrans) (Carbyn 1989; Timm et al. 2004; White and Gehrt 2009) and cougars (Puma bicolor) (Beier 1991). The Australian dingo (Canis dingo) is no exception (Appleby et al. 2017a). In April 2001 this threat was starkly realised when two dingoes on K’gari (Fraser Island), a World Heritage wilderness area off the coast of Queensland, Australia, fatally mauled a young boy. This tragic incident highlighted the need for vigilant supervision of children (Beier 1991; EPA/QPWS 2006; Appleby 2015; Appleby et al. 2017a), a problem that has recently been flagged as a major correlate of attacks elsewhere in the world (Penteriani et al. 2016).

Since 2001, spates of ongoing negative human–dingo interactions (or incidents) on the island, have prompted calls for more effective management interventions. Several years beforehand, Corbett (1998) had begun to identify a suite of actions aimed at bolstering dingo research and management programs, including visitor education programs and erecting warning signage, monitoring interactions, dingo population assessments, radio-tracking dingoes, campground fencing and the lethal removal of dingoes deemed to be problem animals. Corbett (1998) also proposed investigating aversive conditioning, largely with a view to what he envisioned as ‘… educating dingoes to avoid certain areas’ (p. 13). Almost two decades later, most of Corbett’s (1998) recommendations have been widely implemented under the ongoing Fraser Island Dingo Management Strategy (EPA/QPWS 2001; DEHP 2013). These include ongoing, adaptive education and interpretive programs, appropriate campground and human-use area sanitisation and food storage practices, strong enforcement procedures to limit dingoes accessing anthropogenic food, ongoing reinforcement of the vigilant supervision of children and comprehensive dingo and incident-monitoring practices. While there has been some notable success in limiting dingo access to certain areas (e.g. through fencing) and what appears to be a reduction in human-sourced food items found in dingo scats from 46.9% in 1995 to 7.5% most recently (Behrendorff et al. 2016), occasional, serious incidents continue to occur.

A recent review of the most serious incidents recorded on the island highlighted the potential seasonality in incident dynamics, with peaks in March/April and July, corresponding with the dingo breeding and whelping seasons respectively (Appleby et al. 2017a). Most incidents were localised around the townships of Eurong and Happy Valley or the southern barge crossing site of Hook Point/Coolooloi (Appleby et al. 2017a). A strong sex bias was also observed, with 81% of incidents with a reported dingo sex involving males, and 78% of incidents involved juvenile or subadult dingoes (Appleby et al. 2017a). Some 67% of incidents also occurred while people were walking or running. Such patterns may offer an opportunity to focus management effort on key areas, times of year and what may constitute a relatively small number of young dingoes potentially attempting to engage in play behaviour while people walk or run on the beach (Appleby et al. 2017a).

Of all Corbett’s (1998) recommendations, aversive conditioning remains an alluring, but elusive management approach, despite several attempts to date to explore potentially viable options. Aversive conditioning involves the use of stimuli that are painful, noxious or otherwise unpleasant to manipulate the behaviour of animals either through punishment (a reduction in behaviour) or avoidance learning (Dorrance and Gilbert 1977; Gillin et al. 1994; Mazur 2010). Soon after the fatality in 2001, rangers began using sublethal projectiles fired from shotguns and slingshots in an attempt to discourage unwanted dingo behaviours and dingo activity within human-use areas (Allen et al. 2012; EPA/QPWS, 2006). Although few formal records are available, anecdotal reports suggest that the results of these hazing practices were, at best, mixed, with some indication of the approach being more effective on younger, more naïve, dingoes than on older dingoes deemed to be ‘habituated’ (Allen et al. 2012). Allen et al. (2012) defined habituation as ‘… an animal learning to ignore a stimulus (e.g. person) originally seen as threatening through frequent exposure with neutral consequences’ (p. 78). Debates about the role of habituation in negative human–wildlife interactions are ongoing (Appleby et al. 2017a). However, in the context of aversive conditioning, a decreasing response over time to a benign stimulus has important implications for the outcome of some experiments.

Corbett (2009) observed that, rather than hazing leading to dingoes avoiding a particular place, some dingoes had instead learned to move out of sight when a ranger, or ranger vehicle appeared, and likely returned to the area once the rangers had departed (see also Allen et al. 2012). Similar associative confounding has been noted for other species elsewhere (Herrero 2002; Shivik 2006). Because of such problems, hazing in this manner has been largely suspended and the practice has been rarely employed since 2010 (Allen et al. 2012; DEHP 2013). In the most recent comprehensive review of the dingo management strategy on the island, it was recommended that the approach be abandoned in lieu of scientific investigations into its utility, and that other potential approaches be investigated instead (Allen et al. 2012; DEHP 2013).

Several small-scale experimental investigations have also been undertaken. Tauchmann (1999) reported on an assessment of conditioned taste aversion/avoidance (CTA) trials. The study involved lacing food lures (party pies) with either lithium chloride (LiCl) or thiabendazole and placing them at three locations. A reduction in bait consumption was observed at one site only. Edgar et al. (2007) investigated the prospect of a commercially available ultrasonic device in deterring four captive dingoes, but found that the devices had no effect on dingo subjects. Appleby (2015) attempted to examine the responses of four Fraser Island dingoes to electric shock delivered via modified domestic dog shock collars. Only trials involving two dingoes provided reliable results, but these were encouraging in that dingoes receiving shocks immediately ceased exhibiting target behaviours (e.g. making physical contact with a person; approaching a child) and there were hints at a developing avoidance response in one individual and successful deterrence from closely approaching a child in another. How-ever, as has been found in similar studies elsewhere (Hawley et al. 2009, 2013; Shivik et al. 2003), equipment problems led to unreliable collar functionality, ultimately limiting the utility of current designs.

Some objects people carry, such as sticks or walking poles, though better than no protection at all, may largely be ineffective as aversive stimuli, because dingoes rarely get hit (Appleby, pers. obs.). Among 160 serious dingo incident reports collected by Queensland Parks and Wildlife Service (QPWS) (Appleby et al. 2017a) there were seven cases found where people threatened by dingoes were listed as carrying sticks. Cases in which sticks both appeared to deter and not deter dingoes were found. Only one case mentioned a dingo actually being stuck with a stick, an adult male dingo, struck twice by a woman after it repeatedly lunged at her, after which it and two other dingoes left the scene. It is generally unclear how many people actually carry objects such as sticks and how often people carrying these objects have been in threatening interactions with dingoes, making efficacy difficult to assess.

Other activities people undertake in an effort to repel dingoes such as kicking sand, kicking or splashing water, and throwing objects at dingoes, also appear unlikely to repel dingoes in a meaningful way unless perhaps direct contact is regularly made (Appleby et al. 2017b). Clapping and yelling at dingoes may similarly be of little value; however, if used in combination with more immutably aversive stimuli, these could be useful as conditioned warning signals.

There is continuing interest in the pursuit of non-lethal dingo management approaches such as aversive conditioning on Fraser Island (Corbett 2009; Allen et al. 2012; DEHP 2013) and research from elsewhere suggests that a great deal of public support for non-lethal measures exists (Hunt 1984a; Gillin et al. 1994; Gilsdorf et al. 2002; Baker et al. 2005; Massei et al. 2010; Mazur 2010). However, given the mixed or disappointing results of previous attempts at aversive conditioning on Fraser Island mentioned earlier, it is fitting that a comprehensive methodological review be undertaken to determine whether there are approaches capable of offering meaningful benefit. To that end, we aimed at providing a synthesis of relevant explorations of aversive conditioning where similar conflict has driven, and continues to drive, the search for viable, non-lethal management solutions. We were particularly interested in studies involving closely related canid species and other carnivores. We identified eight major categories of aversive conditioning that have been used in various wildlife management contexts, and we briefly summarise each approach, including an assessment of potential advantages and disadvantages. We have also included a pertinent set of empirical investigations for each method. In this regard we selected studies that exemplify particular methodological or experimental strengths and weaknesses from a large array of empirical investigations that were examined in preparation of this review.


Methods

Background

K’gari (Fraser Island) is the world’s largest sand island, located ~200 km north of Brisbane, Australia. It was inscribed onto the World Heritage List in 1992. The Butchulla people are the Traditional Owners of K’gari, and were awarded Native title by the Australian Federal Court in 2014. A mark–recapture study by Appleby and Jones (2011) determined that the island’s dingo population comprised ~130 (range: 104–194) dingoes, but estimates vary, with one study suggesting a population of 162–257 individuals as of the 2012 whelping season (Allen et al. 2015). Approximately 400 000 people are thought to visit the island annually (Allen et al. 2015).

Seriously negative human–dingo interactions (incidents) appear to be quite rare, with Appleby et al. (2017a) reporting an average of 10.7 (range: 3–22) Category E (the most serious) incidents per year between 2001 and 2015. However, despite considerable management effort directed at reducing serious incidents, reporting rates have remained relatively constant across the period (Allen et al. 2015; Appleby et al. 2017a). A single human fatality occurred on the island in April 2001 involving a young boy, but Appleby et al. (2017a) noted that serious incidents involving children continue to occasionally occur. Incident report sample sizes were too small to comprehensively analyse the relevance of proximity of the closest adults to children during interactions; however, Appleby et al. (2017a) suggested the possibility that poorer supervision on the part of adults may have contributed to more severe outcomes in at least some cases, recommending that adults remain within 5 m of children at all times if at all possible. They concluded that despite copious warnings about dingoes in the form of signage and other interpretive materials provided by the QPWS, some visitors may still not recognise the potential threat dingoes can pose, or are otherwise ignoring this threat.

In cases where a dingo is deemed to represent an unacceptable threat to human safety it is usually destroyed, with 110 dingoes destroyed for risk management purposes between 2001 and 2015 (Allen et al. 2015). The impact of lethal control on the dingo population is contentious, with Allen et al. (2015) concluding that it was unlikely to have any adverse impacts on breeding success or sustainability at the current rate, while O’Neill et al. (2017) contended that lethal control on the island leads to social disruption and instability, which in turn leads to negative human–dingo interactions, resulting in a perpetual cycle of conflict.

Literature review

Searches of major scientific literature databases (Scopus, Web of Science, Elsevier, Google Scholar) were conducted in order to locate pertinent literature. Databases maintained by associations producing specific journals were also searched (The Wildlife Society, The Berryman Institute). The primary search terms used were: ‘aversive’, ‘aversive conditioning’, ‘hazing’, ‘nonlethal’, ‘non-lethal’, ‘nonlethal/non-lethal wildlife management’, ‘punishment’, and ‘negative reinforcement’. Specific terminology linked with a particular approach (e.g. ‘conditioned taste aversion/avoidance’) or aversive stimulus (e.g. ‘LiCl’) was also included. Subsequent literature was garnered from references provided within the bibliographies of retrieved documents.

Several other reviews of aversive conditioning and related approaches have been undertaken, largely within the context of livestock depredation management. Each contains valuable insights in relation to non-lethal management approaches, and summaries of 17 major reviews can be found in Table S1, available as supplementary material to this paper. Particular attention is drawn to Baker (2007), who reviewed coyote–human interaction conflict management, and van Bommel and Johnson (2014) as the only major review pertaining directly to dingoes that could be found (again, in the context of livestock depredation management).


Results

In total, 108 empirical studies were found during literature searches. An annotated bibliography of these studies is provided in Table 1. Most studies were drawn from North America (particularly the USA). We derived eight major categories where aversive stimuli were applied in an effort to modify the behaviour of certain animals: CTA, electric fencing, fladry, chemical repellents, fear-evoking stimuli, physical repellents, aversive collars/devices and hard release procedures. A special category of fear-evoking stimuli was predator containment/exclusion using conspecific urine/faeces. Some studies exhibited overlap in terms of these categories. Coyotes specifically featured in 29 studies, wolves in 14, domestic dogs in 9 and dingoes in 3. Four studies involved various predators including (potentially) canids and an additional 17 specifically featured bears. The remaining studies featuring other species contained interesting accounts of success/failure, novel approaches or uses of aversive stimuli or could be considered seminal or otherwise important in a given management context.


Table 1.  A selective summary of 108 empirical studies involving the use of the aversive stimuli
A ‘?’ indicates uncertainty about a figure. A ‘+’ indicates that there are additional elements not included because of space restrictions
Click to zoom

From the assembled literature, a selection was then chosen to exemplify major categories, particularly in terms of reported successes, failures, advantages and disadvantages, covered below.

Conditioned taste aversion/avoidance

Definition/description

Conditioned taste aversion or avoidance (CTA) involves an animal learning that a particular taste, or the ingestion of a particular substance, often in concert with any other salient odour or visual cues, is associated with a feeling of illness (e.g. nausea), thus tending to cause avoidance of consumption of that particular substance in the future (Conover 2002). Such associations can be artificially induced in animals through a variety of mechanisms, but usually involve some sort of chemical being mixed with the ingested substance that produces emesis and nausea (Dorrance and Gilbert 1977; Conover 2002).

Background/overview

Investigations of CTA in relation to wildlife management were first undertaken by Gustavson et al. (1974), who explored whether the emetic, lithium chloride (LiCl), could be used in baits made up of the flesh of selected prey animals or their carcasses and fed to coyote subjects in an effort to induce sickness and suppress attack responses when subjects were later presented with live prey animals. Their initial results, while not entirely definitive, appeared promising (Mason et al. 2001).

Example success

Two notable studies have reported the successful implementation of CTA in relation to preventing wildlife from obtaining anthropogenic food. Cornell and Cornely (1979) treated several food items (e.g. hot dogs) with LiCl and fed them to coyotes attempting to scavenge at a campground, concluding that following observed emesis and related illness, coyote activity was reduced at the campground. Avoidance appeared to last a reasonable amount of time, with coyote activity at the campground remaining low for at least two months, after which the study terminated, but there was no way to determine whether avoidance was directly the result of successful CTA. Treated coyotes were observed to be sick for several hours, and one lay in a campground while recovering. In a second study, Ternent and Garshelis (1999a) demonstrated CTA in black bears that had previously been accessing so-called meals-ready-to-eat (MREs) in a military reserve, with two of three bears exhibiting avoidance more than one year after treatment, while the third bear no longer showed avoidance. The results of Tauchmann (1999) suggested that some dingoes avoided eating baits following trials, but results were confounded because different chemicals were used simultaneously at the same location, making interpretations difficult.

Example failure

Most failures have been reported in relation to studies involving attempts to condition coyotes (or other predators) to avoid killing sheep simply from exposure to baited sheep meat (e.g. Conover et al. 1977; Burns and Connolly 1980; Bourne and Dorrance 1982; Burns 1983b). The majority of criticism appears to relate to what actually constituted an aversion and whether, and to what degree, an aversion to baits would generalise to live prey (Bekoff 1975; Conover et al. 1977; Griffiths et al. 1978; Burns and Connolly 1980; Bourne and Dorrance 1982; Burns 1983a). The latter in particular raises concerns about generalisations beyond a given bait type (e.g. type of food).

Advantages

CTA would generally not require the capture of animals in order to promote conditioning. Theoretically, it could reduce incidence of dingoes seeking food in camp grounds, although this would require a considerable degree of generalisation to be effective (see below).

Disadvantages

The association between particular anthropogenic food and the onset of illness is required for each food type if avoidance of that food is to develop. For example, Ternent and Garshelis (1999a) demonstrated that bears conditioned to avoid MREs did not generalise this response to other foods, even when these other foods were presented along with (but separate from) MREs in post-treatment trials. Dorrance and Gilbert (1977) suggested that, for black bears, CTA would likely be ineffective in producing aversion to tourist handouts and garbage, precisely because of the wide array of tastes and related cues associated with such food. This sort of caveat could logically be extended to many species in similar circumstances, including dingoes on Fraser Island. Also, if treatment results in prolonged malaise, this could be considered unacceptable to members of the public, and could place animals in jeopardising situations that they might otherwise not be in (e.g. they could be vulnerable to injury from people or other animals). If subjects are potentially dangerous animals, people being able to closely approach them, when normally this wouldn’t be possible, might also put such people at risk, particularly if animals are irritable or feel threatened. Finally, there are certain food types that are both naturally and artificially available (e.g. fish) to dingoes. These would need to be excluded from treatment programs, because dingoes would be unlikely to discriminate between artificial sources and natural sources and so would likely come to avoid both.

Conclusion

Given the limitations and disadvantages identified above, CTA is unlikely to be appropriate for managing dingoes on Fraser Island.

Electric fencing

Definition/description

Electric fences usually consist of several electrically conductive wires strung between poles or posts secured to the ground. As the name suggests, electric fences feature a purposeful aversive stimulus in the form of electricity, usually delivered as a pulse at relatively high voltages (e.g. >5000 V) and varying amperages (see, for example, Linhart et al. 1982). There are considerable variations in relation to permanency, materials and energizers used to create the electrical stimulus.

Background/overview

Electric fences are amongst the most widespread attempts to utilise an aversive stimulus in controlling the behaviour of animals, including wild animals. An early example in wildlife management was an investigation into keeping bears from raiding apiaries (Storer et al. 1938a), but until relatively recently, had not been comprehensively explored as a means of a barrier in other applications and for other predators (Dorrance and Bourne 1980).

Example success

Recently, Smith and Gookin (unpubl. data) explored the utility of highly portable electric fencing for campers and hikers to prevent ingression of bears (and other animals) at campsites. They found that as long as food, garbage and related scents/odours were properly managed, bears were successfully deterred from campsites. A video (https://www.youtube.com/watch?v = Sv2G-aRDvyY) of some of the testing involving captive brown bears demonstrates the reluctance that bears appear to exhibit in relation to breaching small areas in which attractive food items were placed, protected by the temporary electric fencing. Smith (pers. comm.) suggests that pricing ranges from US$75 to US$250, depending on requirements, and that the systems are light enough to be highly portable. Davies and Rockwell (1986) reported similar success with reasonably inexpensive, semipermanent electric fencing in deterring polar bears from a research facility. They added that careful management of attractants and proper grounding of the system likely served to deter repeated intrusion attempts from bears, at least one of which was known to exhibit nuisance behaviour in the area.

Example failure

Thompson (1979), who tested 34 types of electric and non-electric fences on penned coyotes, concluded that electric fences were unlikely to serve as effective barriers to coyotes. One important variable that Thompson (1979) identified was the height of fences, with an apparent minimum threshold height of 168 cm, the point at which most coyotes would hit fences if attempting to jump over them. Thompson’s (1979) poor results in relation to electric fencing largely appeared to relate to the difficulty of consistently delivering shock to coyotes attempting to cross fence lines (only 13 of 466 test occasions), with coyotes seemingly able to avoid the wires that delivered shock, and he suggested that shock alone may not be sufficient to deter coyotes with a history of killing sheep. However, given the ability Thompson (1979) noted for coyotes to avoid electrical wire, more appropriate wire spacing for a given species appears to be of relevance.

Advantages

Electric fences effectively exclude animals from human-use areas, and can usually be built cost effectively. Portable, personal fencing is relatively cheap and can be used anywhere that camping is allowed. In addition, conditioned avoidance is likely to be generalised to any similar-looking fence, particularly if salient visual stimuli are also used (e.g. fladry, see next section).

Disadvantages

The electrical stimulus can also affect people so ample warning, and possibly an intermediate barrier may be required to minimise exposure to human beings. Careful consideration needs to be given to designing fences to prevent digging under, or rapid movement through, wiring (Acorn and Dorrance 1994). Fences become ineffective if power is lost. Solar-battery systems will be required for semipermanent or permanent fences in remote areas. If built surrounding entire human-use areas, entry/exit points for people and vehicles will usually have to be included (individual tent sites are probably an exception). In turn, this may limit scalability.

Conclusion

Both semipermanent and temporary, personal electric fencing may have application on Fraser Island; the former, for example, in relation to established camping sites used regularly by commercial operators, and the latter for casual visitors.

Fladry and electrified (turbo) fladry

Definition/description

Fladry is a curious form of fencing or visual barrier constituting a wire (or similar) rope hung between stakes or posts secured to the ground, with material ‘flags’ or pennant-shaped objects suspended at regular intervals along the rope. In this sense it is often used in contexts where a temporary barrier might be required, such as a so-called ‘night-pen’ to protect livestock being moved around. Similar to electric fencing, electrified fladry involves the addition of an electrical stimulus delivered through the fladry materials. This pairing of electric shock with fladry is sometimes referred to as ‘turbo fladry’ (e.g. Bangs et al. 2006).

Background/overview

Fladry was originally used in eastern Europe and Russia to herd wolf packs into constricted areas using a funnel-shape arrangement, where they could then more easily be killed (Okarma and Jędrzejewski 1997; Musiani and Visalberghi 2001). Indeed, it is wild canids, and particularly wolves, that appear most troubled by fladry, the reasons for which are somewhat enigmatic. The apparent wariness and unsettled behaviour often displayed by wolves when close to fladry is reminiscent of aspects of neophobia (van Bommel and Johnson 2014), or the fear of something novel (but see Musiani and Visalberghi 2001).

Example success

Lance et al. (2010) carried out experiments of electrified fladry in penned and field trials involving wolves. From penned trials, they concluded that it was superior to ordinary fladry in protecting a food source, but noted that hunger was a strong motivator in wolves approaching fladry. The authors pointed out that as wolves began to investigate the systems (e.g. nipping and biting at flags), ordinary fladry soon failed, whereas electrified fladry ‘… reinforced the initial fear response’ (Lance et al. 2010, p. 712). They concluded that the initial increase followed by a decrease in approaches observed in relation to electrified fladry were the result of conditioned avoidance. In the final phase of testing, wolves that had been previously presented with electrified fladry were presented with ordinary fladry in post-treatment trials, which eventually they breached, suggesting that avoidance responses would extinguish over time in the absence of shock.

Example failure

Davidson-Nelson and Gehring (2010) found that while electrified fladry was an effective, temporary barrier to wild wolves on protected pastures, it did not deter wild coyotes, suggesting that the aversive effects of fladry may be limited to wolves (see also Okarma and Jędrzejewski 1997). However, Davidson-Nelson and Gehring (2010) wondered whether the lack of effect on coyotes might be a limitation associated with the spacing of flags as well as species and individual differences in initial responses and subsequent habituation.

Advantages

Fladry may be more cost-effective and simpler to erect in comparison to other types of fencing, particularly where temporary, small-scale barriers are required. In conjunction with electrification, or in addition to electric fencing, flags or pennants may serve as a useful, secondary, visual stimulus.

Disadvantages

While fladry can be effective as a barrier, its effects appear to be relatively short-lived, with Musiani et al. (2003) observing a period of up to 60 days before wolves in field trials eventually crossed the barrier, appearing to do so through a learning process of exploratory interactions with various elements of the fladry equipment. In other words, whatever apprehension or fear fladry initially causes, this eventually dissipates over time and exposure. No published tests involving dingoes could be found.

Conclusion

Fladry may have application on Fraser Island, particularly in terms of visually enhancing electric fencing. However, before adoption in management programs, it requires further testing, both as a temporary aversive stimulus in its own right, and also as a secondary, visual stimulus in relation to enhancing associative properties of electric fences

Chemical repellents

Definition/description

Smith et al. (2000, p. 305) defined a repellent as anything designed to ‘… discourage an approaching or attacking animal from completing that approach’. Certain chemicals are irritating or painful (i.e. are in some way aversive) when in contact with mucosal tissues, the nasopharyngeal system, or nerve endings. As such, such chemicals are capable of producing a repellent effect, a good example being capsaicin (or related derivatives). A common delivery mode is via pressurised canisters or containers.

Background/overview

In an attempt to find potential repellents to brown bears and polar bears, commonly implicated in attacks, Miller (1983b) tested a variety of stimuli on captive bears of both species, including a variety of chemicals and chemical sprays. Miller (1983b) found that several chemical sprays had a repellent effect, particularly a commercially available spray marketed as a dog repellent (Halt! Animal Repellents, Inc.), which contained capsaicin. Later, Rogers (1984) tested the same product on free-ranging black bears and an initial focus of the research was to determine whether pain-inducing stimuli would make bears more aggressive or dangerous in realistic settings where encounters may occur. Rogers (1984) found that sprayed bears never acted aggressively, and when sprayed, usually immediately retreated a short distance, where they then rubbed their eyes with their paws.

Example success

Smith et al. (2008) amassed all available (83) records on the use of capsaicin spray (commonly referred to as bear spray) against bears in Alaska, USA. They found that in all cases (72 of 83) where bear spray was used defensively by a person, neither bears nor people were severely injured (i.e. people did not require hospitalisation). In 18% of cases involving aggressive bears, a resumption of the behaviour that prompted spraying occurred again, such that people had to respray bears, which eventually resulted in bears leaving. Wind, although having some effect on spray direction, did not appear to hamper the ability of users to make facial contact of spray with bears. In four cases, spray caused minor injury to people and they had to vacate the area. Smith et al. (2008) concluded that bear spray was an effective non-lethal measure in diffusing potentially dangerous encounters between all three North American bear species and humans, and encouraged widespread use for both human safety and bear conservation reasons.

Example failure

Smith (1998) warned against the use of chemical sprays as some form of topical deterrent to areas or objects, as bears he observed were actually attracted to the scent and displayed vigorous rubbing and body rolling in affected areas (see also Smith et al. 2008). The same could also be true for some other potential irritants, such as citronella.

Advantages

Chemical repellents that visitors could carry and use in appropriate cases (e.g. if threatened or attacked) offer a means of personal, defensive protection that is currently unavailable.

Disadvantages

Capsaicin-based and related sprays are restricted and are illegal to carry in many Australian states and territories, including Queensland (Western Australia is an exception), so appropriate alternatives would need to be found and tested (see below). Certain sprays can also affect people in the process of deployment (Herrero and Higgins 1998; Smith et al. 2008) so care and practice is required in terms of safe operation.

Conclusion

Chemical repellents, particularly in the form of pressurised sprays offer potential in terms of personal and defensive protection for people encountering dingoes that could represent an imminent risk to human safety. However, given legal restrictions, an alternative to capsaicin-based sprays would need to be found and experimentally tested.

Fear-evoking stimuli

Definition/description

This approach encompasses a relatively large array of stimuli that promote or elicit fear. Conover (2002) regarded fear-provoking or -evoking stimuli as those objects that increase an animal’s fear or wariness and include visual (e.g. predator models/effigies, strobing lights, lasers), auditory (e.g. alarm calls, sirens, loud bangs from pyrotechnics or gas exploders) and olfactory (e.g. predator urine) stimuli. So-called ‘frightening devices’, which are often limited to producing visual and auditory stimuli, are synonymous with fear evocation and the allied concept of neophobia.

Background/overview

Effigies, such as the humble ‘scare-crow’, have been utilised in an attempt to protect crops for centuries (Conover 2002), taking advantage of the fear that some species have of being close to human beings (e.g. because humans are a potential threat). Modern versions include rapidly inflating (via compressed air or other gas) human-like effigies that appear periodically (e.g. Stickley et al. 1995; Andelt et al. 1999; Conover 2002) or when activated by motion such as an animal moving nearby (e.g. VerCauteren et al. 2003b). In an attempt to deter coyote depredation (and that of other species) on livestock farms, early incarnations of light- and sound-based frightening devices suggested considerable promise (Linhart 1984).

Example success

Darrow and Shivik (2009) explored the differential effects of light and sound stimuli on captive coyotes, testing each sensory type of stimulus separately, and then in combination. The combined treatment was the only one in which habituation to stimuli was not observed. They also found that light was the more important of the two stimuli, with less habituation observed in light-only treatments than in sound-only treatments. However, and perhaps most interestingly, they also found something akin to an individual personality effect in their results, with individuals that they had classified as bolder more commonly exhibiting habituation to such stimuli, while those classified as shy never exhibited habituation to the stimuli and always showed avoidance. A final class, labelled as persistent coyotes, were initially repelled but did not avoid stimuli, landing them somewhere in the middle of bold and shy.

Example failure

Ultrasonic ‘deterrents’ generally appear to have little merit in actually deterring test subjects in a meaningful manner (Bomford and O’Brien 1990; Romin and Dalton 1992; Bender 2003; Edgar et al. 2007). An exception was Blackshaw et al. (1990), who observed partial success in deterring dogs exposed to various sound-producing stimuli, including ultrasound, at a distance of 1 m. Generally, there was no clear pattern in relation to the frequency (sonic or ultrasonic) or other characteristics (e.g. sound pressure level) of the units tested that produced favourable responses considered consistent with the stimuli being aversive. In the one case where a specific unit produced notable aversions in 9 of 14 (64.3%) dogs exposed, because the unit ranged in frequency of emissions between 17.5 and 55 kHz (i.e. between sonic and ultrasonic frequencies), it was unclear whether it was ultrasound that actually produced the favourable results.

Advantages

Potentially, a very large array of lights and sounds with considerable variation in relation to how they are presented (e.g. randomly modulating pitch, random strobing etc.) is available in developing fear-evoking devices. Much of the equipment required to generate the stimuli is also commonly available and is not likely to be overly expensive. The ability to randomly generate unpredictable combinations of lights and sounds may decrease the prospect of habituation and maintain startle or fear-evoking responses (but see Koehler et al. 1990; Avery 1997; Gilsdorf et al. 2002). Interestingly, Karp et al. (2014) found that signal non-linearity and unpredictability were a key to limiting habituation in conspecific responses to the alarm calls of other meerkats (Suricata suricatta), hinting at how habituation might be avoided in other contexts. Radio-activated systems can limit operation to only those occasions when instrumented (e.g. radio-collared) animals are relatively close to equipment (Breck et al. 2002).

Disadvantages

A common assessment of many fear-evoking or frightening devices is that they work for only short periods and responses are subject to habituation (Koehler et al. 1990; Andelt et al. 1997; Smith et al. 2000; Mason 2001; Conover 2002; Gilsdorf et al. 2002). Extremely loud noises or bright lights (e.g. lasers) can also cause permanent eyesight or hearing damage to both dingoes and people, so care is required in explorations of efficacy.

Conclusion

The sentry-like frightening devices often used in livestock protection applications are probably of limited value on Fraser Island. However, similar hand-held devices that utilise bright, strobe or pulsing lights and multiple, loud sounds emitted in an unpredictable manner might have merit in terms of personal repellents and should be investigated.

Physical repellents (e.g. sublethal projectiles)

Definition/description

In a similar vein to chemical spray repellents, there is a suite of physical or tactile stimuli available that may act as aversive stimuli in many contexts. These include sublethal projectiles (e.g. plastic/rubber bullets, bean bag rounds, paint balls) deployed from firearms, or clay balls/marbles deployed from slingshots.

Background/overview

Most early research using sub-lethal projectiles has involved the three North American bear species. Most of this research was published as internal government reports or was otherwise not accessible for review. An exception was a report by Stenhouse (1983), in which 257 free-living polar bears were attracted to field sites using food lures and exposed to various potentially aversive stimuli including rubber batons and plastic slugs. Stenhouse (1983) reported that rubber batons fired from an antiriot gun were 100% successful in repelling bears, while rubber batons fired from a pistol and plastic slugs failed to repel bears. Later research from Gillin et al. (1994) attempted to pair an otherwise benign stimulus (a bird call not from the area) with an imminent presentation of sub-lethal projectiles to free-living, female brown bears, with somewhat encouraging but ultimately mixed results, leading them to suggest that evidence relating to conditioning of the benign stimulus was inconclusive. Overall, they felt that the use of sub-lethal projectiles was of temporary benefit in managing bear behaviour, but they noted that if site (or place) avoidance was to occur, presentation of the aversive stimulus was required at each site (i.e. experiences at one site were not generalised to all such sites).

Example success/example failure

With the exception of Stenhouse (1983), all empirical examples that could be found, all involving bears, reported mixed results from the use of projectiles (Gillin et al. 1994; Rauer et al. 2003a; Beckmann et al. 2004; Leigh and Chamberlain 2008; Mazur 2010), so only one, relatively recent, example is detailed here. A comprehensive assessment comes from Mazur (2010), who employed a variety of aversive stimuli, including several sub-lethal projectiles, in attempting to modify the behaviour of black bears labelled as food-conditioned. Over a four-year period, she amassed a large dataset of trials involving 150 bears and over 1000 attempts at conditioning. For a subset of 36 food-conditioned bears, rubber slugs fired from a shotgun caused bears to run away on 92% of occasions, which was markedly more successful than other treatments. Mazur (2010) considered conditioning trials to have been most effective on bears that were not considered food-conditioned. Yet there was an indication that at least some bears deemed to be food-conditioned and persistent in terms of food seeking, did undergo beneficial behavioural modification. For instance, 25 of 36 bears were considered less persistent in terms of food-seeking activity (but still displayed unwanted behaviour). Of these, 16 were exposed to treatments for a year or more, and ceased entering human-use areas. A further two animals went from having their behaviour deemed to be unacceptable to acceptable by managers. And of 11 of 36 bears classified as the most persistent in exhibiting food-seeking and unwanted behaviours, one stopped visiting human-use areas altogether and four had their behaviour modified but required treatments each year. The remaining six bears were deemed to represent a danger and were killed or relocated, but it is possible that this number might have been higher without the aversive interventions. Mazur (2010) estimated that at approximately US$400 per bear annually, costs for lethal control and aversive conditioning were roughly comparable.

Advantages

Most physical stimuli such as rubber bullets are likely to be broadly aversive to most individuals encountered and habituation to these sorts of stimuli would probably be uncommon. In other words, such stimuli are likely to be immutably aversive. Proficient users could probably deploy stimuli over longer ranges than most other repellents, although safety considerations may be a limiting factor in this regard.

Disadvantages

The use of firearms is heavily restricted and will necessarily be limited to a few licenced carriers within the wildlife management or law enforcement agencies present on Fraser Island, limiting application. Perhaps the biggest limitation relates to the likelihood of animals developing an avoidance response only in regard to people (and associated stimuli such vehicles) that deploy such stimuli rather than the more ideal response of avoiding a particular place (e.g. a campground) where aversive events take place (Herrero 2002; Shivik et al. 2003; Corbett 2009; Mazur 2010; Appleby 2015). Finally, there is a risk of permanent injury or worse if an animal (or person) is hit by a projectile in certain places (e.g. an eye).

Conclusion

There may be some potential applications for the use of physical aversive stimuli in a limited number of circumstances, most likely presented by highly skilled practitioners. However, due to considerable limitations, alternative approaches should take experimental precedence as recommended in the Allen et al. (2012) review and this approach should generally be avoided.

Aversive collars

Definition/description

Aversive collars consist of an aversive stimulus (commonly shock or a potential irritant such as citronella or lemon spray) that is activated via a radio signal or once a threshold decibel is reached (i.e. from barking or similar vocalisations). Currently all available collars are derived from domestic applications which include so-called ‘virtual fences’ for containing domestic animals, usually through a short-range radio transmission wire being buried around a perimeter within which a collared animal must remain, or else for wider training applications in which the stimulus is activated via a hand-held remote control.

Background/overview

The earliest investigation of shock collars in relation to aversive conditioning in a wildlife management context also remains one of the most compelling. Linhart et al. (1976) tested a custom-built shock collar on the behaviour of four captive coyotes. The experiment consisted of coyotes being presented with black or white rabbits as a food source; however, if coyotes attempted to attack black rabbits, they would receive a shock, whereas no shock was presented if they attacked white rabbits. One coyote failed to learn the association between colour of the rabbit and shock and initially avoided all rabbits, but only for a short time. The remaining three coyotes acquired the association after only 3–5 shock presentations. They retained this response for 3–9 months after treatment without subsequent shock treatments. In a later experiment, Andelt et al. (1999) observed similar avoidance responses rapidly develop in two wild-caught coyotes in relation to lambs, even observing submissive behaviour being exhibited by one conditioned coyote directed at lambs in close proximity.

Example success

Rossler et al. (2012) remains the largest-scale trial of shock collars on free-ranging, wild wolves, with 10 collared subjects, and four control subjects, from a mixture of control and treatment packs, with encouraging results. They compared the time that collared and non-collared wolves spent near bait sites outfitted with radio systems that activated collars when they came into range. Results showed that collared wolves spent significantly less time near bait sites than did control wolves, for periods greater than 40 days. Interestingly, they also found that non-collared pack members in packs containing collared wolves also spent significantly less time at bait sites in comparison to control-only wolf packs, likely facilitated through some process of cultural transmission. Collared wolves were not observed to move outside of established home ranges following treatment. Rossler et al. (2012) concluded that collared wolves had undergone avoidance learning, coming to associate the general area of bait sites with the prospect of shock, thus learning to avoid such places. It is also possible that a similar avoidance response developed in some non-collared wolves within the same packs.

Appleby (2015) reported mixed but potentially beneficial results in modifying the behaviour of two Fraser Island dingoes.

All empirical assessments of citronella collars found related to attempts to suppress barking in domestic dogs, with mixed, but largely beneficial results in this regard (Juarbe-Diaz and Houpt 1996; Wells 2001; Moffat et al. 2003; Sargisson et al. 2012). Sargisson et al. (2012) did note that some dogs grew indifferent to the citronella stimulus, which they considered to indicate that it was only a mildly aversive stimulus.

Example failure

Shivik et al. (2003) presented the results of several experiments in which a variety of non-lethal management techniques were explored. Amongst these, shock collars were tested on 10 penned wolves in an attempt to examine if stimulation would prevent the consumption of a protected food source. They compared results of food consumed over several one-hour tests with a control group of wolves, and in contrast to other studies involving shock collars that had been largely successful, they found no significant difference in consumption levels between groups, although consumption did appear to be lower for collared wolves compared with control wolves. They concluded that shock collars were difficult to use with wolves, showed considerable variation in terms of the responses to stimulation exhibited by individuals, required a great deal of maintenance and suggested that, in conjunction with other logistical constraints, such challenges would complicate incorporation into wider management programs (see also Shivik 2006).

Advantages

Highly precise and contiguous presentation of the shock stimulus is possible in relation to target behaviour, as is precise adjustment of stimulus characteristics such as intensity and duration, which in turn addresses the need to establish stimulus sufficiency (Appleby 2015). Shock may be largely immutable as an aversive stimulus in most cases.

Disadvantages

Animals must be captured and outfitted with shock (or related aversive) collars. Collar performance has been mixed and unreliable in several studies involving wild canids (Shivik et al. 2003; Hawley et al. 2009, 2013; Appleby 2015). While responses to electrical stimulation may vary to some degree within and between individuals (Shivik et al. 2003), variation is more likely due to inconsistent collar performance (Hawley et al. 2009, 2013). Citronella might only be mildly aversive and thus, responses could be subject to habituation over time or muted in certain individuals (see Sargisson et al. 2012). At the time of writing, no commercially available collar systems ideally suited to wildlife management situations are available (but see Hawley et al. 2013 in regard to custom modifying a domestic dog shock collar in an effort to make it more suited to such applications). Polsky (2000) and Overall (2007a, 2007b) raised concerns about shock-induced aggression and other undesirable behavioural changes arising from the use of shock collars on domestic dogs, discussed later.

Conclusion

Shock collars have been investigated in several studies involving similar canids (coyotes and wolves) with largely positive results in relation to conditioning success and the development of avoidance responses. The results of Appleby’s (2015) trials, while minimal, were also promising, and involved Fraser Island dingoes directly. Citronella collars do not appear to have been investigated in relation to an analogous wildlife management context, so more extensive, experimental investigations would be required in this regard. Aversive collars are probably best suited to very specific, challenging cases of conflict.

Hard release procedures

Definition/description

For some animals, capture and handling can represent a potentially aversive experience, which opens up the possibility of this being used specifically in relation to management. The deliberate enhancing of frightening, uncomfortable or otherwise unpleasant aspects of capture and handling processes, and particularly at the time of an animal’s release, is sometimes referred to as hard release (Mazur 2010). A range of aversive stimuli can potentially be used at the point of release, but possible inclusions are loud sounds, bright strobe or flashing lights, and chemical/physical repellents.

Background/overview

It has been recognised for some time that capture and handling can be aversive to some animals that feature in conflict behaviour such as bears (Jonkel 1994; Chi et al. 1998; Clark et al. 2002). Two of the bear studies mentioned in the section on physical repellents actually involved hard release procedures (Beckmann et al. 2004; Leigh and Chamberlain 2008). The results of each were mixed, but in different ways, and here we have chosen to highlight the differing results in relation to place avoidance.

Example success

Leigh and Chamberlain (2008) wanted to ascertain whether bears labelled as a nuisance could be discouraged from exhibiting such behaviour through hard release procedures. They used two treatments: one involving rubber buckshot (fired from a shotgun) as an aversive stimulus, and the other using the same buckshot, but with the addition of dogs for harassment, applied when bears were released from culvert traps (similar in principle to a large cage trap, usually on a trailer for easy transport). Some bears were given more than one treatment. The authors reported that 10 bears (from a total of 11) returned to exhibiting nuisance behaviour within five months of exposure to treatments. In regard to experimental aims, the results were largely unsuccessful. However, only one bear returned to its place of capture, possibly indicating that place avoidance had occurred in most of the bears.

Example failure

Beckmann et al. (2004) measured the effectiveness of hard release procedures using various aversive stimuli in terms of the number of days following treatment that it took bears to return to urban patches where conflict was occurring. This was a more direct assessment of whether bears had learned to associate a hard release experience with a particular area, prompting place avoidance, for at least some reasonable period. Because many bears (44 of 62, ~71%) returned to such sites relatively soon after release (≤40 days), the authors concluded that effectiveness was ultimately limited. However, of potential note is that it did not appear that all bears were released in the same areas where they were captured, which may have confounded the learning process.

Advantages

Hard release procedures can be easily incorporated into existing capture and handling programs being undertaken in other management contexts (e.g. population studies – provided physical recapture is not a requirement). This could assist with identifying and targeting animals that are frequenting places where avoidance would be preferable.

Disadvantages

It may be difficult to avoid the stimuli associated with the management staff present at the point of release from overshadowing stimuli associated with the place, if place avoidance is the desired outcome. It could also make treatment animals very difficult to capture in the future.

Conclusion

For very challenging cases, hard release methods could be of use on Fraser Island in an effort to discourage dingo activity in particular areas (i.e. place avoidance) although aversive stimuli would likely need to be presented remotely (e.g. via remote-controlled shock collars) or in some other way that limits exposed dingoes from associating the experience with management staff, rather than the place of release.


Potential applications of aversive conditioning on Fraser Island

It is apparent from the literature that while there are few, unequivocal successes in relation to the use of aversive stimuli in successfully modifying the behaviour of wild animals, certain approaches warrant further investigation. Not all studies have involved species closely related to dingoes (e.g. wolves and coyotes as opposed to bears), so this needs to be considered in determining appropriate approaches worth trialling. However, similar types of conflict as seen on Fraser Island have led to empirical investigations that illustrate the challenges and opportunities of certain approaches, offering insights in how best to proceed. We determined that there are three major contexts within which empirical investigations of aversive conditioning, garnered from research elsewhere, could be useful in managing human–dingo conflict on Fraser Island. These were:

  1. excluding dingoes from accessing human-use areas such as camping grounds;

  2. diffusing encounters between dingoes and people in which people perceive a threat, or where dingoes actually display threatening or aggressive behaviour, but also in cases where discouraging close-quarter interactions in young or naïve dingoes may aid in preventing more serious conflict from emerging in the future; and

  3. modifying the behaviour of dingoes that chronically exhibit undesirable behaviour.

Opportunities and challenges for utilising aversive stimuli in managing dingoes in each of these contexts will be briefly discussed in the following sections.

Dingo exclusion

Much work has already been done in regard to exclusion on Fraser Island, with conventional fencing being erected around several major townships/resorts as well as some day-use areas and campgrounds (Corbett 2009; Allen et al. 2012). However, there remain a relatively large number of places where people visit or camp that are problematic or prohibitive to fence in this manner. An alternative option to conventional fencing is to explore variations of electric fencing, several of which appear very promising. For instance, there is currently a camping area where such an electric fence is being trialled, with preliminary reports suggesting that the approach is effectively excluding dingoes from the electrified fenced area (Novak, pers. comm.).

We recommend that any site utilising electrified fencing be closely monitored for dingo activity in an effort to both record general activity levels, but also to allow observations of any attempts by dingoes to breach fencing and their responses to the shock stimulus in this regard. Trail cameras capable of day and night (infrared) video recording positioned around the fence line are the best means of collecting such information, but opportunistic interviews with people who were present during such events could also be useful. If possible, several independent control sites should also be established (i.e. a similar fence is erected but not electrified) for comparative purposes. Alternatively, control (or pretreatment) periods for any future sites should be undertaken (i.e. a fence is erected but not activated for a period of ~2–4 weeks) before establishing a treatment period (i.e. the fence is activated for a similar period). A similarly timed post-treatment period would be of use in establishing the degree of conditioning that might have occurred, or whether dingoes can tell the difference between active and inactive fences. Randomised periods of active and inactive fence operation could also be experimentally effective in this regard. This approach should be replicated at as many sites as practicable, but a minimum of six sites is recommended.

An additional treatment or treatment level should be considered, by implementing fladry at some (again, with consideration of adequate replication) or all sites, before the electrification treatment, as fladry may initially cause an apprehension to approach in dingoes. It may also provide an additional, salient visual stimulus to dingoes that they come to associate with shock in the future if they approach and make contact with the fence. It could also discourage attempts to dig under electric fence wire. Post-treatment assessments should also be undertaken to examine the longevity of any effects. Fladry should initially be constructed following the recommendations of Young et al. (2015), who suggested that flags should be attached to wire via circular rings or else knotted below the attachment point, and constructed from a heavy material such as marine vinyl. In addition, Musiani and Visalberghi (2001) suggested that flags should be spaced ≤50 cm apart, bright in colour (consider red), and strung on wire between 25 and 75 cm (e.g. 50 cm) off the ground. We note though that dogs see certain colours poorly so this too may be a worthwhile experimental variable (C. Wynne pers. comm.). Given that most testing has been done on wolves, and the subsequent size differences with dingoes, some modification in design might be required.

In a similar vein, trials should be conducted to examine whether portable electric fences can adequately exclude dingoes from individual camp or tent sites, as Smith and Gookin (unpubl. data) found in relation to bears. It is recommended that, initially, testing be undertaken by management or research personnel, rather than the general public. Pilot test sites should actually be attended so that they are both a realistic analogue to actual campsites, but also in case dingoes that manage to breach fences become trapped, so that the system can be turned off, allowing escape. Some consideration should be given to the presence/absence and availability of attractants (e.g. food lures) as a variable to emulate ‘ideal’ and ‘less-than-ideal’ camp sites. Again, fladry could be included as a secondary stimulus. If initial results are encouraging, recommendations could be made to visitors planning on camping in otherwise unfenced areas to utilise such portable electric fencing, and rates of usage and outcomes should be monitored via direct observation and user surveys. Similarly, control-treatment–post-treatment experiments involving electrified individual storage items such as portable food coolers or bait buckets that might serve as an attractant to dingoes (QPWS, unpubl. data) should be undertaken.

Briefly, the results of Robley et al. (2015) showed promise in potentially excluding dingoes from areas using chemicals derived from or mimicking dominant dingo urine. Theoretically, this could be used to emulate the scent-marking exhibited during territorial maintenance in what Robley et al. (2015, p. 11) called a ‘keep out, no trespassing’ message to other dingoes. These principles have been successfully demonstrated in other species. For example, Jackson et al. (2012) used translocated scent stations (naturally voided scats and urine) from a radio-tracked pack to effectively contain African wild dogs (Lycaon pictus) that had roamed near to or beyond the boundary of a protected area. Similarly, Ausband et al. (2013) found that translocated scent stations (naturally voided scats and urine) successfully manipulated the movements of wolves in one year (2010), but not the following year (2011). They suggested several reasons for a lack of aversion in 2011, including the possibility that additional cues normally associated with territorial maintenance (e.g. howling) may have led to habituation. However, Shivik et al. (2011) found that in coyotes, conspecific urine served as an attractant, with coyotes spending more time in treatment compared to controlled sites.

It is unclear whether bio-fences would be widely applicable on Fraser Island because, usually, relatively small areas such as camp grounds within existing territorial boundaries require exclusion. How readily scent boundaries in such areas would evoke territorially related avoidance is therefore unclear. However, if it did evoke avoidance, the approach is likely to be cheaper than fencing so could be of use in some circumstances, such as excluding dingoes from, for example, Orchid Beach, the only major township left unfenced on the island.

Personal protection

For those rare cases where an encounter involves a dingo or dingoes that could represent an imminent risk to human safety, there is a need for approaches that allow people involved to protect themselves in a safe, appropriate and effective manner using aversive stimuli. However, there is also a case for the general public using such aversive stimuli in another context. Discouraging younger or more naive dingoes from engaging in close-quarter interactions with people, by exposing them to aversive experiences as such behaviour begins to emerge, may also help to prevent more intractable or serious conflict situations involving those animals arising in the future (Mazur 2010).

One collective set of options for managing close-quarter encounters can be grouped into what might be regarded as personal repellents. There are a wide variety of stimuli that could potentially act as repellents. For example, Hunt (1984a) showed that an umbrella rapidly opened in front of charging bears was sufficient to cause them to halt or retreat. However, further provocation during testing caused these bears to recharge or display curiosity (Hunt 1984a). An open, sturdy umbrella may also serve as a useful shield against a dingo making a very close approach, or exhibiting potentially aggressive behaviour, reducing the prospect of a more serious incident occurring. This is similar to the current recommendation by QPWS to carry a stick for self-defence purposes, but may also have some increased utility in this regard. Umbrellas could even be ‘enhanced’ with other potentially aversive light and sound stimuli. However, as it is unclear how dingoes presented with an umbrella, enhanced or not, would actually react, experimental trials would be required before making any recommendations to the public.

A note here that novel stimuli, or previously encountered stimuli used in a novel way can also produce escape and avoidance responses, but if experience with such stimuli is only mildly aversive, responses will rapidly diminish, as has been observed with many fear-evoking stimuli (Koehler et al. 1990; Smith et al. 2000; Conover 2002; Gilsdorf et al. 2002; Shivik et al. 2003; Shivik 2006).

On the other hand, if painful or irritating effects of particular stimuli persist over prolonged periods, this may interfere with the normal, natural behaviours of animals in some cases (e.g. coyotes exposed to LiCl baits: Cornell and Cornely 1979). In some circumstances, a moderate degree of persistence of an aversive effect could be of direct benefit in terms of increasing human safety, as it might allow people the time required to retreat from an aggressive animal (Smith et al. 2008). It is therefore important to contextually determine the ‘sufficiency’ of a stimulus (Appleby 2015). This can be generally regarded as one that effectively and consistently produces a desirable result (e.g. immediate cessation of target behaviour such as an aggressive charge) on repeated occasions across most individuals exposed to the stimulus, but that does not continue to produce aversive effects long after exposure.

Given the restrictions involved with the use of capsaicin-based sprays and sub-lethal projectiles (particularly those fired from firearms) there is a need to find effective, alternative repellents specifically for dingo management on Fraser Island. Appleby et al. (2017b) recently began this process by examining the immediate responses of several dingoes on Fraser Island exposed to three stimuli: a whistle, an emergency air horn and a motorised water pistol. Neither the whistle nor the air horn appeared to be aversive to most dingoes, but the water pistol produced responses consistent with the stimulus being aversive in many cases. However, some dingoes responded very little to any of the stimuli, including the water pistol, and there were hints of habituation to the water pistol stimulus taking place in one subject towards the conclusion of the study. Thus, while the water pistol initially produced repulsion in many cases, this may have been related to neophobia, and water alone may not be sufficient in consistently repelling dingoes exposed on repeated occasions. A potential alternative is a commercially available spray advertised as a dog repellent called SprayShield® (PetSafe), which is listed as containing 1% citronella. No published accounts on the effectiveness of this product as a repellent could be found at the time of writing, so experimental testing is required in order to properly determine efficacy in wildlife settings.

Research such as that of Darrow and Shivik (2009) suggests that light may be an important potentially aversive stimulus to dingoes, or light stimuli in combination with loud sounds, which together may reduce the prospect of habituation (but see Koehler et al. 1990; Avery 1997; Gilsdorf et al. 2002). We are not aware of any studies involving the testing of hand-held light and sound devices in repelling wild animals, and we reiterate that many of the studies that have been undertaken in examining the effects of such stimuli in other contexts report only short-term benefits in modifying animal behaviour. It is possible though that if such a device could be made to resemble something that was more immutably aversive (e.g. if SprayShield® were found to be as effective as some bear sprays appear to be), or if all the stimuli were contiguously used on occasion, beneficial conditioning might occur. In turn, this might reduce the prospect of habituation to the less aversive stimuli, which is often regarded as the biggest limitation of so-called ‘frightening devices’ utilising light and sound stimuli (Koehler et al. 1990; Shivik and Martin 2000; Conover 2002; Gilsdorf et al. 2002; Shivik et al. 2003; Shivik 2006). Variability and unpredictability in relation to visual and audiogenic stimulus characteristics (e.g. non-sequential strobe lights, rapid changes in pitch of sounds, random loud sounds) may also reduce the prospect of habituation (Koehler et al. 1990; Gilsdorf et al. 2002). Suitable test devices will likely need to be custom made.

Remedial aversive conditioning

It is possible that any successful implementation of the previously broached aversive exclusion and repellent measures would go a long way to mitigating much of the ongoing conflict on Fraser Island. However, there still might be cases where dingoes develop behaviour that represents an unacceptable and unavoidable risk to human safety. In the absence of any alternatives, such animals will almost certainly be destroyed. Rather than removing these animals permanently through lethal control, an option is to capture and then house them temporarily in a containment facility, where they could undergo intensive aversive conditioning and assessment procedures (Appleby 2015). This would mirror a similar program involving black bears in North America nicknamed ‘bear school’ (Jonkel 1994: C-22).

There are several other distinct advantages to temporarily containing high-risk animals. The first and most obvious is that it removes any risk posed by such animals to the general public. The second is that aversive conditioning procedures can be undertaken systematically and precisely. Behaviour that is undesirable can be properly defined and then strategically targeted for modification in a stepwise manner, allowing proper conditioning to occur before moving on to the next target behaviour. Aversive conditioning and behavioural assessment procedures could be developed and evaluated in consultation with behavioural experts and animal welfare organisations such as the Royal Society for the Prevention of Cruelty to Animals and the Humane Society (see further below). In cases where behaviour has not been modified to an acceptable degree, decisions about the fate of the animal can then be made – importantly, without the need for haste often required when animals presenting a risk are free-living. In cases where animals are deemed to no longer represent an immediate risk, they could be released (potentially using hard release procedures), having been outfitted with monitoring equipment, and possibly temporary shock collars, so that their whereabouts and behaviour can be assessed and, where necessary, interventions, including the prospect of recapture, can be undertaken after release.


Ethical and welfare considerations

Any aversive conditioning procedure carries with it the potential to impact negatively on the welfare or wellbeing of individuals involved. This is such an important area of discussion that it warrants its own review. As such, we will only touch on some key aspects here, in hope of promoting further discussion and consideration.

The very intention of aversive conditioning is to pair either sets of stimuli or stimuli and behaviour in such a way as to reduce the exhibition of behaviour on the part of animals deemed to be of management concern. As such, this involves deliberately exposing animals to stimuli that cause pain or discomfort or otherwise results in fear, escape and avoidance responses. Note that this potentially involves a very large number of stimuli and stimulus complexes, and is not limited to stimuli that are perhaps more readily equated with pain or discomfort, such as electric shock. Arguably, almost any circumstance that produces punishment must in some way be unpleasant for the animals involved, otherwise the behaviour could continue. This includes stimuli that animals naturally encounter during their lives, such as chemical and physical antipredator strategies of potential prey. Predators that survive negative encounters with potential prey animals undoubtedly learn from such experiences, to such a degree that there is a significant adaptive component to this learning process, whereby, for example, highly visible markings of certain potential prey species serve as an unmistakable warning to would-be predators. Nature is replete with examples, from insects such as ants and wasps, amphibians such as toads, to mammals such as the skunk and the echidna. Some species even co-opt the warning signals (e.g. aposematic colouration) of noxious species despite not being noxious themselves, a phenomenon known as Batesian mimicry.

Modern aversive conditioning is therefore, at its best, simply an extension of these adaptive and learning processes. The goal is to target particular, undesirable behaviours in order to reduce or extinguish their exhibition, while having a limited impact on the general wellbeing or naturalistic behaviour of animals involved. In much the same way that a predator might come to avoid a noxious prey item, aversive conditioning should aim to encourage avoidance of particular behaviours on the part of predators, such as closely approaching humans, while not subverting or modifying normal foraging or other behaviours.

Note that for such a lesson to be quickly and effectively learned, there should be no competing or contradictory information available to the animals. An example would be if interacting with some humans might represent an opportunity to obtain food, while avoiding others (e.g. rangers) means avoiding negative consequences for such behaviour. In other words, behaviour that leads to an animal successfully accessing anthropogenic food is going to be reinforced, which directly competes with the aim of having these animals generalise that close association with humans is neither rewarding nor, in fact, likely to lead to negative outcomes and should therefore be avoided. This kind of discrimination would inevitably undermine the success of any aversive conditioning program and therefore has considerable welfare implications, particularly as it is likely to necessitate a much larger number of exposures to negative or benign encounters with people to extinguish interaction behaviour compared with a situation with no positive reinforcement. For instance, Mech (2017) found that it took ~90 occasions of presenting a handful of dirt to a wolf exposed to previous hand feeding to extinguish food solicitation and related interaction behaviour.

We agree entirely with Lewis et al. (2017, p. 4) when they argue that ‘For managing apex predators, this requires innovative use of the least invasive techniques and translating them into specific actions within conservation efforts’, and Fox and Bekoff (2011, p. 129), who similarly stated that ‘We must ensure that we do everything we can to minimize pain and suffering and cause the least amount of harm’. This is in line with the ethical responsibilities outlined in the National Health and Medical Research Council’s (NHMRC 2013, p. 51) Australian code for the care and use of animals for scientific purposes, which includes the statement ‘Steps must be taken at all times to safeguard the wellbeing of animals by avoiding or minimising harm, including pain and distress, to the animals’. The code also makes provision for experiments in which animals will experience pain and distress that will not be alleviated with two major recommendations (NHMRC 2013, p. 53):

  1. the planned endpoint of the project must be as early as feasible to avoid or minimise pain and distress to the animals,

  2. the animals must be monitored and assessed so that the planned endpoints are detected, and actions must be taken in accordance with the AEC approval for the project.

In the context of aversive conditioning experiments, we take these two recommendations to mean that any approved experiments should be conducted as quickly and efficiently as possible, such that a clear result, either way, is achieved in as short as possible a time frame, and that at all possible times during the experiment, animals involved should be monitored and assessed to ensure their wellbeing is not being compromised. Monitoring would likely entail a mixture of direct observation and behavioural assessment, and where possible and appropriate, the use of animal-borne devices to convey information about movement patterns and activity periods, and to facilitate direct observation.

Further, the code recommends that in relation to specific procedures, they must ‘cause the least harm, including pain and distress, to the animals’ (NHMRC 2013, p. 59). This allows, for example, aversive conditioning procedures to be ranked in relation to potential harm, such that two aversive conditioning procedures that aim to produce the same result (e.g. avoidance responses) can be weighed in terms of the potential harm they may cause. In such cases, obviously the procedure likely to cause the least harm, but have the highest degree of success at behavioural modification, should always be chosen. It also dictates that proposed experiments/approaches should be evaluated and appropriately employed in relation to their assumed aversiveness, an example being that, because the use of an umbrella may be considered less aversive than Sprayshield®, umbrellas should be trialled first and more widely, with the latter being trialled only in cases where either umbrellas are found to be insufficiently effective or for more intractable cases of problem behaviour. It also means that if less aversive procedures are found to be effective, suggestions such as remedial aversive conditioning may not be required, and if required, only in the worst-case scenarios.

The NHMRC (2008) also produces a more specific set of recommendations relating to behaviour modification. This document provides a risk matrix that includes two key areas:

  1. assessment of the frequency and pain associated with a procedure, and

  2. assessment of the consequence of this pain and distress for the animal.

Aversive conditioning procedures are most likely to fall into the ‘frequent’ category for (1), where an animal is highly likely to repeatedly experience pain and distress as a direct result of the procedure. This is the highest category rating. However, well planned and executed experiments should also fall into either the ‘marginal’ (temporary pain and/or distress occurs, but is alleviated in a short time) or ‘negligible’ (minor discomfort occurs for a short time) categories for (2).

Proposed procedures that fall more into the ‘critical’ (permanent impairment and/or serious pain or distress occurs as a result of the procedure) or ‘catastrophic’ (the procedure causes severe pain or distress that cannot be alleviated, which is grounds for euthanasing animals without delay) categories for (2) should obviously be avoided to the greatest degree possible.

Note that this potentially includes several procedures mentioned in this review, such as certain versions of CTA, which have been experimentally shown to cause relatively long periods of pain and distress to animals that have ingested emetic chemicals. Sub-lethal projectiles also have the potential to cause long-term damage and impairment. In the same vein, careful consideration would also obviously need to be given to the details of hard release and remedial aversive conditioning procedures, as previously mentioned. The risks involved in the use of sub-lethal projectiles warrants that only highly trained and skilled professionals be involved in delivering such stimuli. In fact, this recommendation should be extended to any aversive conditioning program that has the potential to fall into the ‘critical’ or ‘catastrophic’ risk categories outlined above. Programs most likely to fall within the ‘marginal’ or ‘negligible’ categories should still be overseen by trained professionals, but may offer opportunities to involve the general public in larger trials of certain defensive stimuli, if pilot studies are successful and provided appropriate information on safety and animal welfare are made available.

The specific case of shock collars

While any application of aversive conditioning might invite controversy, shock collars may be particularly controversial. The use of electric shock carries with it a stigma, not undeserved, perhaps because of historical uses of electric shock in highly invasive human and other animal experiments. It is therefore understandable that any proposed use of electric shock be both carefully considered and ethically defensible. As previously mentioned, there may be less controversial forms of aversive or defensive stimuli that warrant precedence in relation to testing in all but the most intractable cases. However, as Appleby (2015, p. 154) argued, modern ‘… shock collars allow for precise and controllable exposure to the intensity and duration of aversive stimuli’. In turn, this means that the use of shock as an aversive stimulus delivered via modern shock collars is actually well placed to meet the criteria demanded by the NHMRC (2013) code, and the ‘sufficiency’ argument raised above.

However, because of the controversy that shock collars evoke (Appleby 2015), several studies involving domestic dogs have specifically investigated aspects of their impacts on welfare and wellbeing. Results are mixed. A study by Steiss et al. (2007) found no significant changes in plasma cortisol (often used as a physiological measure of stress), adrenocorticotropic hormone (ATCH) or activity levels between three groups of eight dogs exposed to either shock, lemon spray or dummy collars. They did detect a non-significant, but initially marked change (169%) in acute plasma cortisol levels for dogs exposed to aversive stimuli, but this appeared to dissipate relatively quickly.

Conversely, Schilder and van der Borg (2004) found that amongst 32 dogs exposed to a total of 107 shocks, behavioural indications were that shocks were painful and had become associated with the presence of a trainer/owner. They concluded that the welfare of dogs exposed to shocks was at stake. Beerda et al. (1998) examined saliva cortisol and behavioural changes in dogs exposed to a variety of assumed aversive stimuli including shock. They found that random and unpredictable exposure to shock (and certain other stimuli such as loud noises) tended to produce elevations in saliva cortisol levels and a very low posture in exposed dogs.

The latter experiment hints at a potentially important qualifier in the use of aversive stimuli such as shock in relation to causing deleterious effects: that unpredictable, and therefore, inescapable or unavoidable exposure to painful experiences leads to physiological and behavioural indicators of distress in dogs. This was further demonstrated by Schalke et al. (2007), who examined salivary cortisol and heart rate as a measure of stress amongst three groups of dogs shocked via shock collars in different contexts – (1) when they made contact with a prey item (a rabbit dummy), (2) when they failed to return to a trainer after being trained to do so when recalled, and (3) randomly – Group 1 showed no significant increase in saliva cortisol levels, whist the other two groups did. They concluded that dogs that were clearly able to associate their behaviour with the possibility of a shock (i.e. touching a prey item led to a shock) were able to predict and control exposure to the stressor. Similarly, Christiansen et al. (2001) found that shock collars effectively suppressed very close approaches to sheep consistent with attack behaviour in collared dogs, but that apart from the initial reaction to shock, dogs did not show overt signs of stress or anxiety in other contexts.

Thus, it may not simply be shock, in particular, as opposed to other aversive stimuli, that evokes stress responses in dogs, but the lack of predictability about aversive events and a subsequent inability to escape or avoid such events. In turn, carefully providing salient links between behaviour, or predictor stimuli, and aversive events is critical to minimising stress and other related impacts on wellbeing when undertaking aversive conditioning, regardless of the aversive stimulus being utilised.

However, the use of shock collars as a means of attempting to modify the behaviour of domestic dogs remains ardently opposed by some professionals and affiliated organisations, including the Australian Veterinary Association (AVA 2014, p. 118), who released a policy against the use of shock collars: ‘unless it can be scientifically shown that their use does not cause long-term physical or psychological harm to dogs’. Overall (2007a, 2007b) gave a comprehensive summary of concerns about the use of shock collars and provided compelling arguments for how and why other approaches are usually far more appropriate for treating behavioural concerns in domestic dogs. Overall’s (2007a, 2007b) arguments were bolstered, for example, by a report mentioned previously from Polsky (2000), which found that five cases of aggression directed towards humans by domestic dogs may have been prompted by shocks experienced as part of a boundary containment system. Redirected or other forms of aggression were not observed by Appleby (2015) or in other reports of shock collar usage involving wild canids, but this remains a notable concern necessary of consideration.

Shivik et al. (2002) and Schultz et al. (2005) also noted moderate tissue damage localised to the area of some wolves’ necks exposed to shock collar probes. Hawley et al. (2013) found that a revised design involving the use of rounded probes that made contact with the top of the neck (rather than the more usual bottom of the neck) and a reduction in any sharp edges of the collar material itself alleviated some tissue damage. Appleby (unpubl. data) did not observe tissue damage in any shock-collared dingoes examined. Again, though, this is a potential cause for concern and may warrant further collar revision (for example, incorporating retractable probes that only make contact with skin immediately before a shock is delivered). This would also likely serve as a salient secondary stimulus and promote rapid acquisition of escape and avoidance responses.

Understandably, both the AVA (2014) and Overall (2007a, 2007b) are in favour of using positive reinforcement training methods wherever possible. However, methods relying upon positive reinforcement are not likely to be appropriate in modifying the behaviour of wild animals, such as dingoes on Fraser Island, because access to food or other reinforcers is likely to confound and compound incident mitigation. Interestingly, the AVA (2014) does not rule out the use of other behavioural modification collars (e.g. citronella collars) in specific circumstances. It is not immediately clear why exposure to citronella should be any less aversive or more acceptable compared with shock, and, in terms of lingering aversive qualities, may arguably be more aversive.

However, we echo the conclusion of the AVA (2014) and Christiansen et al. (2001) that any proposed use of aversive collars in wildlife management should be reserved for very specific and appropriate cases, and that any trials be conducted by trained and experienced professionals in animal behaviour. Further, we call for the development of a code of practice for the use of aversive conditioning approaches such as shock collars in wildlife management contexts, as a guide for both researchers and Animal Ethics Committees.


Conclusion

No single approach that utilises aversive stimuli in an attempt to modify or moderate the behaviour of potentially dangerous wild animals represents a panacea for conflict resolution. Nor does aversive conditioning more generally. As encounters are capable of occurring across the entire island, and at all times of the day and night, it is also entirely unrealistic to assume that any management authority could prevent all negative encounters from occurring, and neither should it be expected to (Wolfe 2008). Therefore, some degree of risk is always likely to exist in wilderness areas and personal preparedness and responsibility, including adherence to all management regulations and recommendations, remains the best defence in this regard.

We have identified several potential opportunities for aversive stimuli to be incorporated into exclusionary, personal protective and dingo remedial behavioural contexts on Fraser Island, all of which require further, rigorous experimentation. In order to ensure that the wellbeing of dingoes is not compromised, experiments will require approval and oversight of an Animal Ethics Committee. Certain proposed procedures (e.g. remedial aversive conditioning) are also likely to require input and guidance from an expert panel if such measures are ever deemed appropriate and necessary. Even when the outcome of any experiments is encouraging, and leads to an adoption of the approach within the broader management strategy, the best chance of success in reducing conflict is to strategically use such approaches in conjunction with other, critical management approaches. In this sense, management of the ‘human’ element of wildlife–human interactions is as, if not more, important in successfully reducing the prospect of incidents occurring. Provided suitable aversive stimuli and conditioning procedures can be found, they offer an opportunity to fill some gaps in the current management strategy, and could represent a vital alternative to lethal control.


Conflicts of interest

The authors confirm that they have no conflicts of interest in relation to this paper.



Acknowledgements

We extend our thanks to Leisa Masterpon, Jess Mackie, Linda Behrendorff, Dan Novak, Dan Clifton, Moyra McRae, Annie and Dudley Snowden, Michelle Brook, Dave Hopkins, Jason Edgar, Ben Allen and the anonymous reviewers. This study was funded by the Queensland Department of Science, Information Technology and Innovation.


References

Acorn, R. C., and Dorrance, M. J. (1994). An evaluation of anti-coyote electric fences. In ‘Proceedings of the Sixteenth Vertebrate Pest Conference‘. (Eds W. S. Halverson and A. C. Crabb) Paper 2. (University of California: Davis.)

Allen, B., Boswell, J., and Higginbottom, K. (2012). Fraser Island Dingo Management Strategy Review. Report to Department of Environment and Heritage Protection, Ecosure, West Burleigh.

Allen, B., Higginbottom, K., Bracks, J., Davies, N., and Baxter, G. (2015). Balancing dingo conservation with human safety on Fraser Island: the numerical and demographic effects of humane destruction of dingoes. Australasian Journal of Environmental Management 22, 197–215.
Balancing dingo conservation with human safety on Fraser Island: the numerical and demographic effects of humane destruction of dingoes.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Andelt, W. F., Phillips, R. L., Gruver, K. S., and Guthrie, J. W. (1999). Coyote predation on domestic sheep deterred with electronic dog-training collar. Wildlife Society Bulletin 27, 12–18.

Andelt, W. F., Woolley, T. P., and Hopper, S. N. (1997). Effectiveness of barriers, pyrotechnics, flashing lights, and Scarey Man® for deterring heron predation on fish. Wildlife Society Bulletin 25, 686–694.
Effectiveness of barriers, pyrotechnics, flashing lights, and Scarey Man® for deterring heron predation on fish.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Appleby, R. (2015). Dingo–human conflict: attacks on humans. In ‘The Dingo Debate: Origins, Behaviour and Conservation’. (Ed. B. Smith.) pp. 131–158. (CSIRO Publishing: Melbourne.)

Appleby, R., Mackie, J., Smith, B., Bernede, L., and Jones, D. (2017a). Human–dingo interactions on Fraser Island: an analysis of serious incident reports. Australian Mammalogy , .
Human–dingo interactions on Fraser Island: an analysis of serious incident reports.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Appleby, R., Smith, B., Mackie, J., Bernede, L., and Jones, D. (2017b). Preliminary observations of dingo responses to assumed aversive stimuli. Pacific Conservation Biology 23, 295–301.
Preliminary observations of dingo responses to assumed aversive stimuli.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Appleby, R. G., and Jones, D. (2011). Analysis of a preliminary dingo capture–mark–recapture experiment on Fraser Island conducted by the Queensland Parks and Wildlife Service. Environmental Futures Centre, Griffith University.

Ausband, D. E., Mitchell, M. S., Bassing, S. B., and White, C. (2013). No trespassing: using a biofence to manipulate wolf movements. Wildlife Research 40, 207–216.
No trespassing: using a biofence to manipulate wolf movements.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

AVA (2014). Policy compendium: Australian Veterinary Association. Available at: https://www.ava.com.au/sites/default/files/Policy%20Compendium_Jan_2014.pdf [verified 30 October 2017]

Avery, M. L. (1997). Repellents: integrating sensory modalities. In ‘Repellents in wildlife management: proceedings of the second Denver Wildlife Research Center Special Symposium. ‘. (Ed. J. R. Mason.) Paper 3. :  Colorado State University, Fort Collins.)

Baker, R. O. (2007). A review of successful urban coyote management programs implemented to prevent or reduce attacks on humans and pets in southern California. In ‘Proceedings of the 12th wildlife damage management conference ‘. (Eds D. L Nolte, W. M. Arjo and D. H. Stalman) Paper 58. Available at: http://digitalcommons.unl.edu/icwdm_wdmconfproc/58/ [verified 30 October 2017].

Baker, S. E., Ellwood, S. A., Watkins, R., and Macdonald, D. W. (2005). Non-lethal control of wildlife: using chemical repellents as feeding deterrents for the European badger Meles meles. Journal of Applied Ecology 42, 921–931.
Non-lethal control of wildlife: using chemical repellents as feeding deterrents for the European badger Meles meles.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 1:CAS:528:DC%2BD2MXhtF2nsL3P&md5=8dcc3b0d64237e2745a5ebd95cf2fb1eCAS |

Baker, S. E., Johnson, P. J., Slater, D., Watkins, R. W., and Macdonald, D. W. (2007). Learned food aversion with and without an odour cue for protecting untreated baits from wild mammal foraging. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 102, 410–428.
Learned food aversion with and without an odour cue for protecting untreated baits from wild mammal foraging.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Bangs, E., Jimenez, M., Niemeyer, C., Fontaine, J., Collinge, M., Krischke, R., Handegard, L., Shivik, J., Sime, C., and Nadeau, S. (2006). Nonlethal and lethal tools to manage wolf/livestock conflict in the northwestern United States. In ‘Proceedings of the 22nd Vertebrate Pest Conference’. (Eds R. M. Timm and J. M. O’Brien) pp. 7–16. (University of California, Davis.)

Beckmann, J. P., Lackey, C. W., and Berger, J. (2004). Evaluation of deterrent techniques and dogs to alter behavior of “nuisance” black bears. Wildlife Society Bulletin 32, 1141–1146.
Evaluation of deterrent techniques and dogs to alter behavior of “nuisance” black bears.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Beerda, B., Schilder, M. B. H., van Hooff, J. A. R. A. M., de Vries, H. W., and Mol, J. A. (1998). Behavioural, saliva cortisol and heart rate responses to different types of stimuli in dogs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 58, 365–381.
Behavioural, saliva cortisol and heart rate responses to different types of stimuli in dogs.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Behrendorff, L., Leung, L. K.-P., McKinnon, A., Hanger, J., Belonje, G., Tapply, J., Jones, D., and Allen, B. L. (2016). Insects for breakfast and whales for dinner: the diet and body condition of dingoes on Fraser Island (K’gari). Scientific Reports 6, 23469.
Insects for breakfast and whales for dinner: the diet and body condition of dingoes on Fraser Island (K’gari).Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 1:CAS:528:DC%2BC28XkvVCgtL4%3D&md5=5ca02a3580400faca756c73ea3810ce6CAS |

Beier, P. (1991). Cougar attacks on humans in the United States and Canada. Wildlife Society Bulletin 19, 403–412.

Bekoff, M. (1975). Predation and aversive conditioning in coyotes. Science 187, 1096.
Predation and aversive conditioning in coyotes.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 1:STN:280:DC%2BC3cvht1egug%3D%3D&md5=725ea5ed5d8641aed5da4df14f5aedd1CAS |

Bender, H. (2003). Deterrence of kangaroos from agricultural areas using ultrasonic frequencies: efficacy of a commercial device. Wildlife Society Bulletin 31, 1037–1046.
Deterrence of kangaroos from agricultural areas using ultrasonic frequencies: efficacy of a commercial device.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Beringer, J., VerCauteren, K. C., and Millspaugh, J. J. (2003). Evaluation of an animal-activated scarecrow and a monofilament fence for reducing deer use of soybean fields. Wildlife Society Bulletin 31, 492–498.
Evaluation of an animal-activated scarecrow and a monofilament fence for reducing deer use of soybean fields.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Biedenweg, T. A., Parsons, M. H., Fleming, P. A., and Blumstein, D. T. (2011). Sounds scary? Lack of habituation following the presentation of novel sounds. PLoS One 6, e14549.
Sounds scary? Lack of habituation following the presentation of novel sounds.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 1:CAS:528:DC%2BC3MXht1Ohtb0%3D&md5=ba90d9162f2a3d57aa2aed6b75c73754CAS |

Blackshaw, J. K., Cook, G. E., Harding, P., Day, C., Bates, W., Rose, J., and Bramham, D. (1990). Aversive responses of dogs to ultrasonic, sonic and flashing light units. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 25, 1–8.
Aversive responses of dogs to ultrasonic, sonic and flashing light units.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Bomford, M., and O’Brien, P. H. (1990). Sonic deterrents in animal damage control: a review of device tests and effectiveness. Wildlife Society Bulletin 18, 411–422.

Borgo, J. S., Conner, L. M., and Conover, M. R. (2006). Role of predator odor in roost site selection of southern flying squirrels. Wildlife Society Bulletin 34, 144–149.
Role of predator odor in roost site selection of southern flying squirrels.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Bourne, J., and Dorrance, M. J. (1982). A field test of lithium chloride aversion to reduce coyote predation on domestic sheep. Journal of Wildlife Management 46, 235–239.
A field test of lithium chloride aversion to reduce coyote predation on domestic sheep.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Breck, S. W., Williamson, R., Niemeyer, C., & Shivik, J. A. (2002). Non-lethal radio activated guard for deterring wolf depredation in Idaho: summary and call for research. USDA National Wildlife Research Center – Staff Publications, No. 467.

Burns, R. J. (1983a). Coyote predation aversion with lithium chloride: management implications and comments. Wildlife Society Bulletin 11, 128–133.
Coyote predation aversion with lithium chloride: management implications and comments.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Burns, R. J. (1983b). Microencapsulated lithium chloride bait aversion did not stop coyote predation on sheep. Journal of Wildlife Management 47, 1010–1017.
Microencapsulated lithium chloride bait aversion did not stop coyote predation on sheep.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Burns, R. J., and Connolly, G. E. (1980). Lithium chloride bait aversion did not influence prey killing by coyotes. In ‘Proceedings of the 9th Vertebrate Pest Conference‘. (Eds J. P. Clark and R. E. Marsh) Paper 5. (University of California, Davis)

Carbyn, L. N. (1989). Coyote attacks on children in western North America. Wildlife Society Bulletin 17, 444–446.
Coyote attacks on children in western North America.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Cavalcanti, S. M., Crawshaw, P. G., and Tortato, F. R. (2012). Use of electric fencing and associated measures as deterrents to jaguar predation on cattle in the Pantanal of Brazil. In ‘Fencing for Conservation: Restriction of evolutionary potential or a riposte to threatening processes’. (Ed. M. J. Somers and M. W. Hayward) pp. 295–309. (Springer.)

Chi, D. K., Chester, D., and Gilbert, B. K. (1998). Effects of capture procedures on black bear activity at an Alaskan salmon stream. Ursus 10, 563–569.

Christiansen, F. O., Bakken, M., and Braastad, B. O. (2001). Behavioural changes and aversive conditioning in hunting dogs by the second-year confrontation with domestic sheep. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 72, 131–143.
Behavioural changes and aversive conditioning in hunting dogs by the second-year confrontation with domestic sheep.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Clapperton, B. K., Matthews, L. R., Fawkes, M. S., and Pearson, A. J. (1996). Lithium and cyanide-induced conditioned food aversions in brushtail possums. Journal of Wildlife Management 60, 195–201.
Lithium and cyanide-induced conditioned food aversions in brushtail possums.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Clark, J. D., Huber, D., and Servheen, C. (2002). Bear reintroductions: lessons and challenges: invited paper. Ursus 13, 335–345.

Conover, M. R. (1985). Protecting vegetables from crows using an animated crow-killing owl model. Journal of Wildlife Management 49, 643–645.
Protecting vegetables from crows using an animated crow-killing owl model.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Conover, M. R. (1990). Reducing mammalian predation on eggs by using a conditioned taste aversion to deceive predators. Journal of Wildlife Management 54, 360–365.
Reducing mammalian predation on eggs by using a conditioned taste aversion to deceive predators.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Conover, M. R. (2002). ‘Resolving Human–Wildlife Conflicts: the Science of Wildlife Damage Management.’ (CRC Press: Florida.)

Conover, M. R., Francik, J. G., and Don, E. M. (1977). An experimental evaluation of aversive conditioning for controlling coyote predation. Journal of Wildlife Management 41, 775–779.
An experimental evaluation of aversive conditioning for controlling coyote predation.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Conover, M. R., and Lyons, K. S. (2003). Reducing or delaying egg depredation by punishing free-ranging predators for opening eggs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 83, 177–185.
Reducing or delaying egg depredation by punishing free-ranging predators for opening eggs.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Cook, M., and Mineka, S. (1989). Observational conditioning of fear to fear-relevant versus fear-irrelevant stimuli in rhesus monkeys. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 98, 448.
Observational conditioning of fear to fear-relevant versus fear-irrelevant stimuli in rhesus monkeys.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 1:STN:280:DyaK3c%2FnvVynsw%3D%3D&md5=954aa3ff93f980688570ffa9796d457cCAS |

Cooper, D. M., Kershner, E. L., and Garcelon, D. K. (2005). The use of shock collars to prevent island fox (Urocyon littoralis) predation on the endangered San Clemente loggerhead shrike (Lanius ludovicianus mearnsi). Institute for Wildlife Studies, Arcata, CA, USA.

Coppinger, R., Coppinger, L., Langeloh, G., Gettler, L., and Lorenz, J. (1988). A decade of use of livestock guarding dogs. In ‘Proceedings of the Thirteenth Vertebrate Pest Conference’. (Eds A. C. Crabb and R. E. Marsh) Paper 43. (University of California, Davis.)

Corbett, L. K. (1998). ‘Management of Dingoes on Fraser Island.’ (ERA Environmental Services.)

Corbett, L. K. (2009). Audit (2009) of Fraser Island dingo management strategy.

Cornell, D., and Cornely, J. (1979). Aversive conditioning of campground coyotes in Joshua Tree National Monument. Wildlife Society Bulletin 7, 129–131.

D’Angelo, G. J., D’Angelo, J. G., Gallagher, G. R., Osborn, D. A., Miller, K. V., and Warren, R. J. (2006). Evaluation of wildlife warning reflectors for altering white-tailed deer behavior along roadways. Wildlife Society Bulletin 34, 1175–1183.
Evaluation of wildlife warning reflectors for altering white-tailed deer behavior along roadways.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Dale, A. R., Statham, S., Podlesnik, C. A., and Elliffe, D. (2013). The acquisition and maintenance of dogs’ aversion responses to kiwi (Apteryx spp.) training stimuli across time and locations. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 146, 107–111.
The acquisition and maintenance of dogs’ aversion responses to kiwi (Apteryx spp.) training stimuli across time and locations.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Darrow, P. A., and Shivik, J. A. (2009). Bold, shy, and persistent: variable coyote response to light and sound stimuli. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 116, 82–87.
Bold, shy, and persistent: variable coyote response to light and sound stimuli.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Davidson-Nelson, S. J., and Gehring, T. M. (2010). Testing fladry as a nonlethal management tool for wolves and coyotes in Michigan. Human–Wildlife Interactions 4, 87–94.

Davies, J. C., and Rockwell, R. F. (1986). An electric fence to deter polar bears. Wildlife Society Bulletin 14, 406–409.

deCalesta, D. S., and Cropsey, M. G. (1978). Field test of a coyote-proof fence. Wildlife Society Bulletin 6, 256–259.
Field test of a coyote-proof fence.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

DEHP (2013). Fraser Island dingo conservation and risk management strategy. The Queensland State Government.

Dorrance, M. J., and Bourne, J. (1980). An evaluation of anti-coyote electric fencing. Journal of Range Management 33, 385–387.
An evaluation of anti-coyote electric fencing.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Dorrance, M. J., and Gilbert, B. K. (1977). Considerations in the application of aversive conditioning. Test methods for vertebrate pest control and management materials. ASTM Special Technical Publication 625, 136–144.
Considerations in the application of aversive conditioning. Test methods for vertebrate pest control and management materials.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Edgar, J. P., Appleby, R. G., and Jones, D. N. (2007). Efficacy of an ultrasonic device as a deterrent to dingoes (Canis lupus dingo): a preliminary investigation. Journal of Ethology 25, 209–213.
Efficacy of an ultrasonic device as a deterrent to dingoes (Canis lupus dingo): a preliminary investigation.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Ellins, S. R., and Catalano, S. M. (1980). Field application of the conditioned taste aversion paradigm to the control of coyote predation on sheep and turkeys. Behavioral and Neural Biology 29, 532–536.
Field application of the conditioned taste aversion paradigm to the control of coyote predation on sheep and turkeys.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 1:STN:280:DyaL3M%2FosF2qtQ%3D%3D&md5=dcbd4bef884d91a17564ae0320d7c45dCAS |

Ellins, S. R., Catalano, S. M., and Schechinger, S. A. (1977). Conditioned taste aversion: a field application to coyote predation on sheep. Behavioral Biology 20, 91–95.
Conditioned taste aversion: a field application to coyote predation on sheep.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

EPA/QPWS (2001). Fraser Island dingo management strategy. Environmental Protection Agency/Queensland Parks and Wildlife Service, Queensland Government, Brisbane.

EPA/QPWS (2006). Fraser Island dingo management strategy – review. Environmental Protection Agency/Queensland Parks and Wildlife Service, Queensland Government, Brisbane.

Fedderwitz, F. (2010). Protecting dogs against attacks by wolves (Canis lupus), with comparison to African wild dogs (Lycaon pictus) and dholes (Cuon alpinus). M.Sc. Thesis, Linköpings Universitet, Sweden. Available at: http://www.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:322325/fulltext01.pdf

Fox, C. H., and Bekoff, M. (2011). Integrating values and ethics into wildlife policy and management – lessons from North America. Animals (Basel) 1, 126.
Integrating values and ethics into wildlife policy and management – lessons from North America.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Gallagher, G. R., and Prince, R. H. (2003). Negative operant conditioning fails to deter white-tailed deer foraging activity. Crop Protection 22, 893–895.
Negative operant conditioning fails to deter white-tailed deer foraging activity.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Gates, N., Rich, J., Godtel, D., and Hulet, C. (1978). Development and evaluation of anti-coyote electric fencing. Journal of Range Management 31, 151–153.
Development and evaluation of anti-coyote electric fencing.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Gehring, T. M., Hawley, J. E., Davidson, S. J., Rossler, S. T., Cellar, A. C., Schultz, R. N., Wydeven, A. P., and VerCauteren, K. C. (2006). Are viable non-lethal management tools available for reducing wolf-human conflict? Preliminary results from field experiments. In ‘Proceedings of the 22nd Vertebrate Pest Conference, University of California, Davis‘. (Eds R. M. Timm and J. R. O’Brien) pp. 2–6. (University of California, Davis.)

Gillin, C. M., Hammond, F. M., and Peterson, C. M. (1994). Evaluation of an aversive conditioning technique used on female grizzly bears in the Yellowstone ecosystem. Bears: Their Biology and Management 9, 503–512.
Evaluation of an aversive conditioning technique used on female grizzly bears in the Yellowstone ecosystem.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Gilsdorf, J. M., Hygnstrom, S. E., and VerCauteren, K. C. (2002). Use of frightening devices in wildlife damage management. Integrated Pest Management Reviews 7, 29–45.
Use of frightening devices in wildlife damage management.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Gilsdorf, J. M., Hygnstrom, S. E., VerCauteren, K. C., Blankenship, E. E., and Engeman, R. M. (2004a). Propane exploders and electronic guards were ineffective at reducing deer damage in cornfields. Wildlife Society Bulletin 32, 524–531.
Propane exploders and electronic guards were ineffective at reducing deer damage in cornfields.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Gilsdorf, J. M., Hygnstrom, S. E., VerCauteren, K. C., Clements, G. M., Blankenship, E. E., and Engeman, R. M. (2004b). Evaluation of a deer‐activated bio‐acoustic frightening device for reducing deer damage in cornfields. Wildlife Society Bulletin 32, 515–523.
Evaluation of a deer‐activated bio‐acoustic frightening device for reducing deer damage in cornfields.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Greene, R. J. (1982). An application of behavioral technology to the problem of nuisance bears. The Psychological Record 32, 501–511.
An application of behavioral technology to the problem of nuisance bears.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Griffiths, R. E., Jr, Connolly, G. E., Burns, R. J., and Sterner, R. T. (1978). Coyotes, sheep and lithium chloride. In ‘Proceedings of the 8th Vertebrate Pest Conference’. (Eds W. E. Howard and R. E. Marsh) pp. 190–196. (University of California, Davis.)

Gustavson, C. R., Garcia, J., Hankins, W. G., and Rusiniak, K. W. (1974). Coyote predation control by aversive conditioning. Science 184, 581–583.
Coyote predation control by aversive conditioning.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 1:STN:280:DC%2BC3cvgtlemtA%3D%3D&md5=49495f098f82faada5d8b25204f251bdCAS |

Gustavson, C. R., Gustavson, J. C., and Holzer, G. A. (1983). Thiabendazole-based taste aversions in dingoes (Canis familiaris dingo) and New Guinea wild dogs (Canis familiaris hallstromi). Applied Animal Ethology 10, 385–388.
Thiabendazole-based taste aversions in dingoes (Canis familiaris dingo) and New Guinea wild dogs (Canis familiaris hallstromi).Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Gustavson, C. R., Jowsey, J. R., and Milligan, D. N. (1982). A 3-year evaluation of taste aversion coyote control in Saskatchewan. Journal of Range Management 35, 57–59.
A 3-year evaluation of taste aversion coyote control in Saskatchewan.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Gustavson, C. R., Kelly, D. J., Sweeney, M., and Garcia, J. (1976). Prey-lithium aversions. I: Coyotes and wolves. Behavioral Biology 17, 61–72.
Prey-lithium aversions. I: Coyotes and wolves.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 1:STN:280:DyaE283jtFemsQ%3D%3D&md5=11e0a21972656d58fd6d56f19f0e5244CAS |

Hansen, I., Bakken, M., and Braastad, B. O. (1997). Failure of LiCl-conditioned taste aversion to prevent dogs from attacking sheep. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 54, 251–256.
Failure of LiCl-conditioned taste aversion to prevent dogs from attacking sheep.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Hawley, J. E., Gehring, T. M., Schultz, R. N., Rossler, S. T., and Wydeven, A. P. (2009). Assessment of shock collars as nonlethal management for wolves in Wisconsin. Journal of Wildlife Management 73, 518–525.
Assessment of shock collars as nonlethal management for wolves in Wisconsin.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Hawley, J. E., Rossler, S. T., Gehring, T. M., Schultz, R. N., Callahan, P. A., Clark, R., Cade, J., and Wydeven, A. P. (2013). Developing a new shock-collar design for safe and efficient use on wild wolves. Wildlife Society Bulletin 37, 416–422.
Developing a new shock-collar design for safe and efficient use on wild wolves.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Herrero, S. (1970). Human injury inflicted by grizzly bears. The chance of human injury in the national parks can be reduced to a minimum through improved management. Science 170, 593–598.
Human injury inflicted by grizzly bears. The chance of human injury in the national parks can be reduced to a minimum through improved management.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 1:STN:280:DyaE3M%2FgslKksA%3D%3D&md5=09c93aa7d3b851e2aa4c53ade3c83bfdCAS |

Herrero, S. (2002). ‘Bear Attacks: their Causes and Avoidance.’ (The Lyons Press: Guilford, CT.)

Herrero, S., and Fleck, S. (1990). Injury to people inflicted by black, grizzly or polar bears: recent trends and new insights. Bears: Their Biology and Management •••, 25–32.

Herrero, S., and Higgins, A. (1998). Field use of capsicum spray as a bear deterrent. Ursus 10, 533–537.

Herrero, S., and Higgins, A. (1999). Human injuries inflicted by bears in British Columbia: 1960–97. Ursus 11, 209–218.

Herrero, S., and Higgins, A. (2003). Human injuries inflicted by bears in Alberta: 1960–98. Ursus 14, 44–54.

Herrero, S., Higgins, A., Cardoza, J. E., Hajduk, L. I., and Smith, T. S. (2011). Fatal attacks by American black bear on people: 1900–2009. Journal of Wildlife Management 75, 596–603.
Fatal attacks by American black bear on people: 1900–2009.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Hoover, S. E., and Conover, M. R. (1998). Effectiveness of volatile irritants at reducing consumption of eggs by captive coyotes. Journal of Wildlife Management 62, 399–405.
Effectiveness of volatile irritants at reducing consumption of eggs by captive coyotes.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Hoover, S. E., and Conover, M. R. (2000). Using eggs containing an irritating odor to teach mammalian predators to stop depredating eggs. Wildlife Society Bulletin 28, 84–89.

Horn, S. W. (1983). An evaluation of predatory suppression in coyotes using lithium chloride-induced illness. Journal of Wildlife Management 47, 999–1009.
An evaluation of predatory suppression in coyotes using lithium chloride-induced illness.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Hunt, C. (1984a). Vol. 1. Behavioral responses of bears to tests of repellents, deterrents and aversive conditioning. Vol. 2. Deterrents, aversive conditioning and other practices: an annotated bibliography to aid in bear management. Theses, Dissertations, Professional Papers, University of Montana, Paper 7046.

Hunt, C. L. (1984b). Behavioral responses of bears to tests of repellents, deterrents and aversive conditioning. M.Sc. Thesis, University of Montana, Missoula. Available at: http://scholarworks.umt.edu/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?article=8081&context=etd (Paper 7046)

Jackson, C. R., McNutt, J. W., and Apps, P. J. (2012). Managing the ranging behaviour of African wild dogs (Lycaon pictus) using translocated scent marks. Wildlife Research 39, 31–34.
Managing the ranging behaviour of African wild dogs (Lycaon pictus) using translocated scent marks.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Jelinski, D. E., Rounds, R. C., and Jowsey, J. R. (1983). Coyote predation on sheep, and control by aversive conditioning in Saskatchewan. Journal of Range Management 36, 16–19.
Coyote predation on sheep, and control by aversive conditioning in Saskatchewan.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Jonkel, C. (1994). Grizzly/Brown Bears. In ‘The Handbook: Prevention and Control of Wildlife Damage’. Part 30, C17–C23.

Juarbe-Diaz, S. V., and Houpt, K. A. (1996). Comparison of two antibarking collars for treatment of nuisance barking. Journal of the American Animal Hospital Association 32, 231–235.
Comparison of two antibarking collars for treatment of nuisance barking.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 1:STN:280:DyaK283psFamuw%3D%3D&md5=959637b042f7b85aaef17a438870c772CAS |

Karp, D., Manser, M. B., Wiley, E. M., and Townsend, S. W. (2014). Nonlinearities in meerkat alarm calls prevent receivers from habituating. Ethology 120, 189–196.
Nonlinearities in meerkat alarm calls prevent receivers from habituating.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Kimball, B. A., Provenza, F. D., and Burritt, E. A. (2002). Importance of alternative foods on the persistence of flavor aversions: implications for applied flavor avoidance learning. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 76, 249–258.
Importance of alternative foods on the persistence of flavor aversions: implications for applied flavor avoidance learning.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Kistler, C., Hegglin, D., von Wattenwyl, K., and Bontadina, F. (2013). Is electric fencing an efficient and animal-friendly tool to prevent stone martens from entering buildings? European Journal of Wildlife Research 59, 905–909.
Is electric fencing an efficient and animal-friendly tool to prevent stone martens from entering buildings?Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Kloppers, E. L., St. Clair, C. C., and Hurd, T. E. (2005). Predator-resembling aversive conditioning for managing habituated wildlife. Ecology and Society 10, 31.
Predator-resembling aversive conditioning for managing habituated wildlife.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Koehler, A. E., Marsh, R. E., and Salmon, T. P. (1990). Frightening methods and devices/stimuli to prevent mammal damage–a review. Proceedings of the Fourteenth Vertebrate Pest Conference, Paper 50.

Lance, N. J., Breck, S. W., Sime, C., Callahan, P., and Shivik, J. A. (2010). Biological, technical, and social aspects of applying electrified fladry for livestock protection from wolves (Canis lupus). Wildlife Research 37, 708–714.
Biological, technical, and social aspects of applying electrified fladry for livestock protection from wolves (Canis lupus).Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Leigh, J., and Chamberlain, M. (2008). Effects of aversive conditioning on behavior of nuisance Louisiana black bears. M.Sc. Thesis, Louisiana State University.

Lewis, P.-M., Burns, G. L., and Jones, D. (2017). Response and responsibility: humans as apex predators and ethical actors in a changing societal environment. Food Webs 12, 49–55.
Response and responsibility: humans as apex predators and ethical actors in a changing societal environment.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Linhart, S. B. (1984). Strobe light and siren devices for protecting fenced-pasture and range sheep from coyote predation. In ‘Proceedings of the Eleventh Vertebrate Pest Conference’. (Ed. D. O. Clark.) Paper 20. (University of California, Davis)

Linhart, S. B., Dasch, G. J., Johnson, R. B., Roberts, J. D., and Packham, C. J. (1992). Electronic frightening devices for reducing coyote predation on domestic sheep: efficacy under range conditions and operational use. In ‘Proceedings of the Fifteenth Vertebrate Pest Conference’. (Ed. J. E. Borrecco and R. E. Marsh.) Paper 47. ( University of California, Davis.)

Linhart, S. B., Roberts, J. D., and Dasch, G. J. (1982). Electric fencing reduces coyote predation on pastured sheep. Journal of Range Management 35, 276–281.
Electric fencing reduces coyote predation on pastured sheep.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Linhart, S. B., Roberts, J. D., Schumake, S. A., and Johnson, R. (1976). Avoidance of prey by captive coyotes punished with electric shock. In ‘Proceedings of the 7th Vertebrate Pest Conference’. (Ed. W. E. Howard and R. E. Marsh.) Paper 30. (Monterey, California: University of California, Davis)

Linnell, J. D. C., Andersen, R., Andersone, Z., Balciauskas, L., Blanco, J. C., Boitani, L., Brainerd, S. M., Breitenmoser, U., Kojola, I., and Liberg, O. (2002). The fear of wolves: a review of wolf attacks on people. NINA Oppdragsmelding 731. 65 pp.

Maguire, G. S., Stojanovic, D., and Weston, M. A. (2009). Conditioned taste aversion reduces fox depredation on model eggs on beaches. Wildlife Research 36, 702–708.
Conditioned taste aversion reduces fox depredation on model eggs on beaches.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Mason, J. R. (2001). Management alternatives relative to predators. Paper presented at the USDA-APHIS-WS-National Wildlife Research Center, Utah State University, Logan, UT. Available at: http://1l0ro98sq023mvw7b3h3nzc6.wpengine.netdna-cdn.com/files/2010/09/020.pdf

Mason, J. R., and Burns, R. J. (1996). Effectiveness of Vichos non-lethal collars in deterring coyote attacks on sheep. In ‘Proceedings of the 17th Vertebrate Pest Conference’. (Ed. R. M. Timm and C. Crabb.) Paper 6. (: University of California, Davis.)

Mason, J. R., Shivik, J. A., & Fall, M. W. (2001). Chemical repellents and other aversive strategies in predation management. USDA National Wildlife Research Center, Staff Publications, Paper No. 556.

Massei, G., Quy, R. J., Gurney, J., and Cowan, D. P. (2010). Can translocations be used to mitigate human–wildlife conflicts? Wildlife Research 37, 428–439.
Can translocations be used to mitigate human–wildlife conflicts?Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Mazur, R. L. (2010). Does aversive conditioning reduce human–black bear conflict? Journal of Wildlife Management 74, 48–54.
Does aversive conditioning reduce human–black bear conflict?Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

McNay, M. E. (2002). Wolf–human interactions in Alaska and Canada: a review of the case history. Wildlife Society Bulletin 30, 831–843.

Mech, D. (2017). Extinguishing a learned response in a free-ranging gray wolf (Canis lupus). Canadian Field Naturalist 131, 23–25.
Extinguishing a learned response in a free-ranging gray wolf (Canis lupus).Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Miller, G. D. (1983a). Responses of captive grizzly and polar bears to potential repellents. Bears: Their Biology and Management 5, 275–279.
Responses of captive grizzly and polar bears to potential repellents.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Miller, G. D. (1983b). Responses of captive grizzly and polar bears to potential repellents. International Conference on Bear Research & Management 5, 275–279.

Miller, G. D. (1987). Field tests of potential polar bear repellents. Bears: Their Biology and Management 7, 383–390.
Field tests of potential polar bear repellents.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Moffat, K. S., Landsberg, G. M., and Beaudet, R. (2003). Effectiveness and comparison of citronella and scentless spray bark collars for the control of barking in a veterinary hospital setting. Journal of the American Animal Hospital Association 39, 343–348.
Effectiveness and comparison of citronella and scentless spray bark collars for the control of barking in a veterinary hospital setting.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Musiani, M., Mamo, C., Boitani, L., Callaghan, C., Gates, C. C., Mattei, L., Visalberghi, E., Breck, S., and Volpi, G. (2003). Wolf depredation trends and the use of fladry barriers to protect livestock in western North America. Conservation Biology 17, 1538–1547.
Wolf depredation trends and the use of fladry barriers to protect livestock in western North America.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Musiani, M., and Visalberghi, E. (2001). Effectiveness of fladry on wolves in captivity. Wildlife Society Bulletin 29, 91–98.
Effectiveness of fladry on wolves in captivity.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Nass, R. D., and Theade, J. (1988). Electric fences for reducing sheep losses to predators. Journal of Range Management 41, 251–252.
Electric fences for reducing sheep losses to predators.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

NHMRC (2008). Guidelines to promote the wellbeing of animals used for scientific purposes: the assessment and alleviation of pain and distress in research animals. National Health and Medical Research Council, Canberra. Available at: http://www.nhmrc.gov.au.

NHMRC (2013). Australian code for the care and use of animals for scientific purposes. National Health and Medical Research Council, Canberra.

Nolte, D. L., VerCauteren, K. C., Perry, K. R., and Adams, S. E. (2003). Training deer to avoid sites through negative reinforcement. In ‘Proceedings of the 10th Wildlife Damage Management Conference’. (Ed. K. A, Fagerstone and G. W. Witmer) pp. 95–104. (University of Nebraska, Lincoln.)

O’Neill, A. J., Cairns, K. M., Kaplan, G., and Healy, E. (2017). Managing dingoes on Fraser Island: culling, conflict, and an alternative. Pacific Conservation Biology 23, 4–14.
Managing dingoes on Fraser Island: culling, conflict, and an alternative.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Okarma, H., and Jędrzejewski, W. (1997). Livetrapping wolves with nets. Wildlife Society Bulletin 25, 78–82.

Olsen, A., and Lehner, P. N. (1978). Conditioned avoidance of prey in coyotes. Journal of Wildlife Management 42, 676–679.
Conditioned avoidance of prey in coyotes.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Osborn, F. V. (2002). Capsicum oleoresin as an elephant repellent: field trials in the communal lands of Zimbabwe. Journal of Wildlife Management 66, 674–677.
Capsicum oleoresin as an elephant repellent: field trials in the communal lands of Zimbabwe.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Overall, K. L. (2007a). Considerations for shock and ‘training’ collars: concerns from and for the working dog community. Journal of Veterinary Behavior: Clinical Applications and Research 2, 103–107.
Considerations for shock and ‘training’ collars: concerns from and for the working dog community.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Overall, K. L. (2007b). Why electric shock is not behavior modification. Journal of Veterinary Behavior: Clinical Applications and Research 2, 1–4.
Why electric shock is not behavior modification.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Penteriani, V., del Mar Delgado, M., Pinchera, F., Naves, J., Fernández-Gil, A., Kojola, I., Härkönen, S., Norberg, H., Frank, J., and Fedriani, J. M. (2016). Human behaviour can trigger large carnivore attacks in developed countries. Scientific Reports 6, .
Human behaviour can trigger large carnivore attacks in developed countries.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 1:CAS:528:DC%2BC28XitFanurc%3D&md5=201ed1a160c73ab6448d093325b45b88CAS |

Phillips, R. B., and Winchell, C. S. (2011). Reducing nontarget recaptures of an endangered predator using conditioned aversion and reward removal. Journal of Applied Ecology 48, 1501–1507.
Reducing nontarget recaptures of an endangered predator using conditioned aversion and reward removal.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Polsky, R. (2000). Can aggression in dogs be elicited through the use of electronic pet containment systems? Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 3, 345–357.
Can aggression in dogs be elicited through the use of electronic pet containment systems?Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Rathore, A. K. (1984). Evaluation of lithium chloride taste aversion in penned domestic dogs. Journal of Wildlife Management 48, 1424.
Evaluation of lithium chloride taste aversion in penned domestic dogs.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 1:CAS:528:DyaL2MXnsVWrsQ%3D%3D&md5=7c710f9afa278a6860a42319eb0bbbd3CAS |

Rauer, G., Kaczensky, P., and Knauer, F. (2003). Experiences with aversive conditioning of habituated brown bears in Austria and other European countries. Ursus 14, 215–224.

Reidy, M. M., Campbell, T. A., and Hewitt, D. G. (2008). Evaluation of electric fencing to inhibit feral pig movements. Journal of Wildlife Management 72, 1012–1018.
Evaluation of electric fencing to inhibit feral pig movements.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Robley, A., Lindeman, M., Cook, I., Woodford, L., and Moloney, P. (2015). Dingo semiochemicals: towards a non-lethal control tool for the management of dingoes and wild dogs in Australia. Available at: https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Alan_Robley/publication/290195212_Dingo_Semiochemicals_Towards_a_non-lethal_control_tool_for_the_management_of_dingoes_and_wild_dogs_in_Australia/links/569579c608aeab58a9a4ec2f/Dingo-Semiochemicals-Towards-a-non-lethal-control-tool-for-the-management-of-dingoes-and-wild-dogs-in-Australia.pdf

Rogers, L. L. (1984). Reactions of free-ranging black bears to capsaicin spray repellent. Wildlife Society Bulletin 12, 59–61.

Romin, L. A., and Dalton, L. B. (1992). Lack of response by mule deer to wildlife warning whistles. Wildlife Society Bulletin 20, 382–384.
Lack of response by mule deer to wildlife warning whistles.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Rossler, S. T., Gehring, T. M., Schultz, R. N., Rossler, M. T., Wydeven, A. P., and Hawley, J. E. (2012). Shock collars as a site-aversive conditioning tool for wolves. Wildlife Society Bulletin 36, 176–184.
Shock collars as a site-aversive conditioning tool for wolves.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Sargisson, R. J., Butler, R., and Elliffe, D. (2012). An evaluation of the Aboistop citronella-spray collar as a treatment for barking of domestic dogs. ISRN Veterinary Science 2011, .
An evaluation of the Aboistop citronella-spray collar as a treatment for barking of domestic dogs.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Schalke, E., Stichnoth, J., Ott, S., and Jones-Baade, R. (2007). Clinical signs caused by the use of electric training collars on dogs in everyday life situations. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 105, 369–380.
Clinical signs caused by the use of electric training collars on dogs in everyday life situations.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Schilder, M. B. H., and van der Borg, J. A. M. (2004). Training dogs with help of the shock collar: short and long term behavioural effects. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 85, 319–334.
Training dogs with help of the shock collar: short and long term behavioural effects.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Schultz, R. N., Jonas, K. W., Skuldt, L. H., and Wydeven, A. P. (2005). Experimental use of dog-training shock collars to deter depredation by gray wolves. Wildlife Society Bulletin 33, 142–148.
Experimental use of dog-training shock collars to deter depredation by gray wolves.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Seamans, T. W., and VerCauteren, K. C. (2006). Evaluation of ElectroBraid™ fencing as a white-tailed deer barrier. Wildlife Society Bulletin 34, 8–15.
Evaluation of ElectroBraid™ fencing as a white-tailed deer barrier.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Shivik, J. A. (2006). Tools for the edge: what’s new for conserving carnivores. Bioscience 56, 253–259.
Tools for the edge: what’s new for conserving carnivores.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Shivik, J. A., Asher, V., Bradley, L., Kunkel, K., Phillips, M., Breck, S., and Bangs, E. (2002). Electronic aversive conditioning for managing wolf predation. In ‘Proceedings of the 20th Vertebrate Pest Conference’. (Ed. R. M. Timm.) pp. 227–231. ( University of Nebraska, Lincoln.)

Shivik, J. A., and Martin, D. J. (2000). Aversive and disruptive stimulus applications for managing predation. In ‘Proceedings of the Ninth Wildlife Damage Management Conference, ,’. (Ed. M. C. Brittingham, J. Kays and R. McPeake.) pp. 111–119. (Pennsylvania State University: Pennsylvania.)

Shivik, J. A., Treves, A., and Callahan, P. (2003). Nonlethal techniques for managing predation: primary and secondary repellents. Conservation Biology 17, 1531–1537.
Nonlethal techniques for managing predation: primary and secondary repellents.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Shivik, J. A., Wilson, R. R., and Gilbert-Norton, L. (2011). Will an artificial scent boundary prevent coyote intrusion? Wildlife Society Bulletin 35, 494–497.
Will an artificial scent boundary prevent coyote intrusion?Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Sillings, J. L., Tomas, T. N. Jr, and Forbes, J. E. (1989). Demonstration electric fences to control black bear damage to apiaries in New York State. In ‘Fourth Eastern Wildlife Damage Control Conference’. (Ed. S. R. Craven and S. Nichols) pp. 142–146. (USDA/APHIS/ADC, Sun Prairie, Wisconsin.)

Smith, M. E., Linnell, J. D., Odden, J., and Swenson, J. E. (2000). Review of methods to reduce livestock depredation. II. Aversive conditioning, deterrents and repellents. Acta Agriculturae Scandinavica, Section A – Animal Science 50, 304–315.
Review of methods to reduce livestock depredation. II. Aversive conditioning, deterrents and repellents.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Smith, T. S. (1998). Attraction of brown bears to red pepper spray deterrent: caveats for use. Wildlife Society Bulletin 26, 92–94.

Smith, T. S., Herrero, S., Debruyn, T. D., and Wilder, J. M. (2008). Efficacy of bear deterrent spray in Alaska. Journal of Wildlife Management 72, 640–645.
Efficacy of bear deterrent spray in Alaska.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Steiss, J. E., Schaffer, C., Ahmad, H. A., and Voith, V. L. (2007). Evaluation of plasma cortisol levels and behavior in dogs wearing bark control collars. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 106, 96–106.
Evaluation of plasma cortisol levels and behavior in dogs wearing bark control collars.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Stenhouse, G. B. (1983). Bear detection and deterrent study. Department of Renewable Resources, Yellowknife, Northwest Territories. Cape Churchill, Manitoba.

Stickley, A. R., Mott, D. F., and King, J. O. (1995). Short-term effects of an inflatable effigy on cormorants at catfish farms. Wildlife Society Bulletin 23, 73–77.
Short-term effects of an inflatable effigy on cormorants at catfish farms.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Storer, T. I., Vansell, G. H., and Moses, B. D. (1938). Protection of mountain apiaries from bears by use of electric fence. Journal of Wildlife Management 2, 172–178.
Protection of mountain apiaries from bears by use of electric fence.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Tauchmann, A. (1999). Aversive conditioning trial on dingoes using lithium chloride and thiabendazole. Unpublished report, The University of Queensland, Gatton.

Ternent, M. A., and Garshelis, D. L. (1999). Taste-aversion conditioning to reduce nuisance activity by black bears in a Minnesota military reservation. Wildlife Society Bulletin 27, 720–728.

Thompson, B. C. (1979). Evaluation of wire fences for coyote control. Journal of Range Management 32, 457–461.
Evaluation of wire fences for coyote control.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Timm, R. M., Baker, R. O., Bennett, J. R., and Coolahan, C. C. (2004). Coyote attacks: an increasing suburban problem. In ‘Transactions of the North American Wildlife and Natural Resources Conference’. (Eds R. M. Timm, and W. P. Gorenzel.) pp. 67–68. (Wildlife Management Institute: Washington, DC.)

van Bommel, L., and Johnson, C. (2014). Protecting livestock while conserving ecosystem function: non-lethal management of wild predators. In ‘Carnivores of Australia. Past, Present and Future’. (Eds A. S. Glen, and C. R. Dickman.) pp. 323–353. (CSIRO Publishing: Melbourne.)

van Bommel, L., and Johnson, C. N. (2012). Good dog! Using livestock guardian dogs to protect livestock from predators in Australia’s extensive grazing systems. Wildlife Research 39, 220–229.
Good dog! Using livestock guardian dogs to protect livestock from predators in Australia’s extensive grazing systems.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

VerCauteren, K. C., Hygnstrom, S. E., Pipas, M. J., Fioranelli, P. B., Werner, S. J., and Blackwell, B. F. (2003a). Red lasers are ineffective for dispersing deer at night. Wildlife Society Bulletin 31, 247–252.
Red lasers are ineffective for dispersing deer at night.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

VerCauteren, K. C., Lavelle, M. J., and Moyles, S. (2003b). Coyote-activated frightening devices for reducing sheep predation on open range. USDA National Wildlife Research Center, Staff Publication No. 285.

VerCauteren, K. C., Shivik, J. A., and Lavelle, M. J. (2005). Efficacy of an animal-activated frightening device on urban elk and mule deer. Wildlife Society Bulletin 33, 1282–1287.
Efficacy of an animal-activated frightening device on urban elk and mule deer.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Wagner, K. K., and Nolte, D. L. (2001). Evaluation of Hot Sauce® as a repellent for forest mammals. Wildlife Society Bulletin 28, 76–83.

Wells, D. L. (2001). The effectiveness of a citronella spray collar in reducing certain forms of barking in dogs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 73, 299–309.
The effectiveness of a citronella spray collar in reducing certain forms of barking in dogs.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

White, L. A., and Gehrt, S. D. (2009). Coyote attacks on humans in the United States and Canada. Human Dimensions of Wildlife 14, 419–432.
Coyote attacks on humans in the United States and Canada.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Wolfe, M. L. (2008). Avoiding the blame game in managing problem black bears. Human–Wildlife Interactions 82.

Young, J. K., Miller, E., and Essex, A. (2015). Evaluating fladry designs to improve utility as a nonlethal management tool to reduce livestock depredation. Wildlife Society Bulletin 39, 429–433.
Evaluating fladry designs to improve utility as a nonlethal management tool to reduce livestock depredation.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Zarco‐González, M., and Monroy‐Vilchis, O. (2014). Effectiveness of low‐cost deterrents in decreasing livestock predation by felids: a case in central Mexico. Animal Conservation 17, 371–378.
Effectiveness of low‐cost deterrents in decreasing livestock predation by felids: a case in central Mexico.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Zemlicka, D., and Mason, J. (2000). Response of captive coyotes to Renardine coyote repellent. USDA National Wildlife Research Center, Staff Publication No. 805.