Register      Login
Animal Production Science Animal Production Science Society
Food, fibre and pharmaceuticals from animals
REVIEW (Open Access)

Welfare of beef cattle in Australian feedlots: a review of the risks and measures

Hannah E. Salvin https://orcid.org/0000-0002-6851-1849 A D , Angela M. Lees https://orcid.org/0000-0003-4898-2843 B C , Linda M. Cafe A , Ian G. Colditz B and Caroline Lee B
+ Author Affiliations
- Author Affiliations

A NSW Department of Primary Industries, Livestock Industries Centre, Trevenna Road, University of New England, Armidale, NSW 2351, Australia.

B CSIRO Agriculture and Food, Animal Behaviour and Welfare, FD McMaster Laboratory, Locked Bag 1, Armidale, NSW 2350, Australia.

C Present address: School of Environmental and Rural Science, Trevenna Road, University of New England, Armidale, NSW 2351, Australia.

D Corresponding author. Email: hannah.salvin@dpi.nsw.gov.au

Animal Production Science 60(13) 1569-1590 https://doi.org/10.1071/AN19621
Submitted: 5 November 2019  Accepted: 11 June 2020   Published: 10 July 2020

Journal Compilation © CSIRO 2020 Open Access CC BY-NC-ND

Abstract

The rising global demand for animal protein is leading to intensification of livestock production systems. At the same time, societal concerns about sustainability and animal welfare in intensive systems is increasing. This review examines the risks to welfare for beef cattle within commercial feedlots in Australia. Several aspects of the feedlot environment have the potential to compromise the physical and psychological welfare of cattle if not properly monitored and managed. These include, but are not limited to, animal factors such as the influence of genetics, temperament and prior health, as well as management factors such as diet, pen design, resource provision, pregnancy management, and stock-person attitudes and skills. While current industry and producer initiatives exist to address some of these issues, continuous improvements in welfare requires accurate, reliable and repeatable measures to allow quantification of current and future welfare states. Existing measures of welfare are explored as well as proxy indicators that may signal the presence of improved or reduced welfare. Finally, potential future measures of welfare that are currently under development are discussed and recommendations for future research are made.

Additional keywords: affective state, behaviour, health, stress, temperament.

Introduction

Dramatic increases in global food demand have resulted in growing pressure on the agricultural sector to improve efficiency of production and to do so in an environmentally and ethically sustainable manner (Tilman et al. 2002). In coming years, it is predicted that livestock production will continue to intensify to support the increasing global demand for animal protein (Nardone et al. 2010; Henry et al. 2012). For many centuries, cattle husbandry in parts of northern Europe relied on housing and hand-feeding animals for extended periods of the year in habitations shared with humans or in barns, and it is a practice that continues today. Close confinement with intensive feeding of cattle in open air feedlots was developed as a method for raising cattle commercially in Australia in the 1960s (Gaughan and Sullivan 2014). Feedlots today are intensive beef-production systems designed to finish cattle on high-energy concentrate diets so as to meet various market specifications. Commercial feedlots can be found around the world and their scale and the proportion of the beef market they occupy varies considerably across countries. In Australia, there are ~400 accredited cattle feedlots, with an average capacity of 2793 cattle per feedlot (Australian Lot Feeders Association 2015). At any one time, ~1.2 million head are located within feedlots (~4% the total Australian cattle population), where they spend, on average, 95 days, equating to the last 10–15% of their lifespan (Australian Lot Feeders Association 2015). The increasing capacities of feedlots will result in increasing pressure on producers to monitor, maintain and improve the welfare of cattle in these systems.

Societal concerns regarding animal welfare are increasing, which is driving increased scrutiny of livestock industries (Wilson et al. 2002a; Eurobarometer 2007; Spain et al. 2018). Livestock producers tend to emphasise the importance of health and access to suitable resources such as food, water and shelter when considering welfare, while consumers place an additional emphasis on freedom to move and the ability to display natural behaviours (Vanhonacker et al. 2008; Phillips et al. 2009). The deviation of feedlots away from the more ‘natural’ environment offered by extensive systems may, therefore, lead to consumer concerns around the welfare of cattle within feedlots.

Since the emergence of animal welfare as an issue of importance (Brambell 1965) there have been considerable improvements in breeding, management, transport and slaughter practices in farm animals across most production systems (Fraser 2008; Farm Animal Welfare Council 2009). Outside of legislative enforcement, improvements in animal-welfare standards within a production system must rely on producer, industry or retailer drivers. Regardless of the impetus, an improvement in welfare within any system requires, first, an understanding of the potential issue and, then, a way of objectively measuring an outcome so as to determine the effectiveness of any intervention.

It is acknowledged that there are similar, but also different challenges to welfare for cattle in feedlots and pasture-based production systems. However, a direct comparison of the welfare risks presented by intensive versus extensive cattle production is not within the scope of the present review. For details of some of the welfare issues presented by risks inherent in extensive cattle production systems, see the review by Petherick (2005). It is also noteworthy that the feedlot environment may protect cattle against several welfare hazards experienced in other production systems, such as hunger, predation and some types of parasitism. Further, increased levels of monitoring achieved in feedlots enables many problems to be identified and treated earlier than some extensive systems allow. A discussion of these benefits over extensive systems is not included in the review as it will not assist in the development of plans to monitor and improve welfare in feedlots. Overall, it can be concluded that there are strengths and weaknesses of both extensive and intensive production systems, with one not necessarily superior to the other. Where insufficient research is available in beef cattle, research in dairy cattle is occasionally used, and is identified as such. For a review of the welfare issues affecting intensive beef production in the United States, see Tucker et al. (2015).

The present review aims to identify aspects of the Australian feedlot environment that may compromise both the physical and psychological welfare of beef cattle and that need to be carefully monitored and managed. Grandin (2016) described muddy conditions, heat stress and animal handling as the three major welfare concerns for outdoor feedlot cattle. While these are important factors, additional animal, resource and management factors may also affect cattle welfare (Colditz et al. 2014). In many areas, there are producer or industry plans in place to manage many of these risks, and research into methods of monitoring and improving welfare continues (for examples, see the Australian Lot Feeders Association website1). The present review aims to aid in the development of such plans and inform future research by providing a comprehensive overview of the main threats to animal welfare in feedlots. The review will focus on the Australian system and the identification of existing, practical welfare measures that may aid in continued improvement of welfare. Proxy indicators that are not direct measures of welfare, but which may imply the presence of improved or reduced welfare are also presented as well as new measures, which are currently limited to the experimental setting. Finally, future directions for research are suggested.


Factors affecting welfare in the feedlot

Several aspects of the feedlot environment have the potential to compromise physical and psychological welfare if not properly monitored and managed. The present review attempts to identify these aspects and outline the literature, which supports the conclusion that welfare may be affected.

Physical factors

Health

Injury and illness are well established causes of reduced welfare in cattle. While feedlot cattle are inspected daily, the identification of ill health by stockpersons remains challenging (Rezac et al. 2014). The effectiveness of stockperson training programs and observational skills in identifying sick or injured animals is, therefore, a predominant limiting factor to maintaining welfare (Weary et al. 2009; Daigle and Ridge 2018). Morbidities within feedlot enterprises tend to be associated with bovine respiratory disease (Sackett et al. 2006; Cusack et al. 2007; Edwards 2010), metabolic disorders, particularly acidosis (Galyean and Rivera 2003), and heat stress (Hahn and Mader 1997; Hahn 1999; Entwistle et al. 2000; Sackett et al. 2006). The incidence of ill health typically decreases as days spent in the feedlot increase (Snowder et al. 2006), although Vogel et al. (2015) found that the greatest percentage of mortalities occurred in the mid-feeding period (from 31 days on feed to 61 days before processing). A 2010 health survey of the Australian feedlot sector found that 84% of morbidities were related to respiratory issues, while 11% involved the musculoskeletal system and included, for example, lameness caused by foot and hoof injuries and septic arthritis (Perkins 2013).

The lifetime health history of cattle may also influence welfare within feedlots. Diagnosis and treatment of illnesses both before and after feedlot entry may have a lasting influence on the health status and overall performance of individual animals while in the feedlot (Brown-Brandl et al. 2006a). Cattle that had been clinically diagnosed and treated for illnesses before entering the feedlot spent 23% less time at the feed bunk than did cattle that had not been previously treated for health concerns (Sowell et al. 1997). Brown-Brandl et al. (2006a) reported that animals with a history of pneumonia had higher respiration rates (average 10.5% increase) and lower average daily gains (1.46 ± 0.04 kg/day versus 1.54 ± 0.02 kg/day) than did cattle that had no history of pneumonia. Similarly, Gardner et al. (1999) highlighted the differences in the average daily gain of steers with no (1.58 kg/day), inactive (1.43 kg/day) and active (1.17 kg/day) respiratory-tract lesions. Reinhardt et al. (2012) indicated that as the number of times that cattle were treated for any cause of morbidity increased, there was a linear decrease in average daily gains (P < 0.01). Therefore, identifying and treating sick or injured animals as well as understanding their health history before feedlot entry, is important for improving not only the welfare but also the performance of feedlot cattle. In reality, accurate health-history records may not be feasible if cattle are purchased through the saleyards or have had multiple owners, so direct purchasing from producers who keep good health records is preferential. Improved health will reduce the cost of gain in meeting market specifications, while also minimising exposure to other aspects of the feedlot environment that may affect welfare.

Thermal comfort

All homeothermic animals have a thermoneutral zone where the animal experiences a thermal environment that supports optimal health and productivity (Ames 1980). Thermal comfort incorporates a lower critical temperature and an upper critical temperature (Yousef 1985), below and above which additional energy is required to maintain body homeostasis. When animals encounter climatic conditions outside their thermoneutral zone, the immediate response is self-preservation, where energy is diverted away from growth and shifted towards maintaining homeostasis and core body temperature (Baumgard and Rhoads 2012). However, it is important to recognise that core body temperature in cattle is not static and exhibits a circadian rhythm (Bitman et al. 1984; Robertshaw 1985; Lefcourt et al. 1999). These variations in core body temperature can be considered as the balance between heat energy produced or accumulated and heat energy dissipated from the body (Hahn 1999; Mader and Davis 2004; Gaughan et al. 2008b). This accumulation and dissipation of heat from the body is constantly adjusting; however, as heat load conditions increase, heat accumulation can become greater than dissipation, leading to accumulated heat load in cattle and an inability to regulate core body temperature (Mader et al. 2006; Gaughan et al. 2008b). Chronic exposure to heat load can be present in many regions throughout the world during the summer months and can be a major stressor for healthy feedlot cattle (Gaughan et al. 2013). Climatic conditions that influence the heat load placed on cattle include ambient temperature, relative humidity, solar radiation, wind speed and rainfall (Bond et al. 1967; Blackshaw and Blackshaw 1994; Brown-Brandl et al. 2006b).

Environmental conditions are not the only factors that influence thermal exchange in livestock. Genotype (e.g. Bos indicus content) and point of origin (e.g. tropical or temperate climate) influence the phenotype expressed by cattle, particularly coat characteristics (Belasco et al. 2015). For example, cattle with a short summer coat have a lower critical temperature of 15°C compared with –7.8°C for those with a long dense winter coat (Brandle et al. 1994). The selection of appropriate genetics for the production environment is, therefore, important for the continual improvement of animal welfare (Colditz et al. 2014). For feedlot cattle, other animal factors that influence thermal balance include the number of days on feed, body condition (fat coverage and deposition), growth rate, health status, and adaptation to the feedlot environment and local climate (Mader 2003; Maia et al. 2005; Gaughan et al. 2008b; Dikmen et al. 2012). The heat increment of feed provided, that is the increase in body temperature experienced after eating, may also influence the heat load placed on cattle. Fats, carbohydrates and protein have low heat increments, while fibre has a high heat increment (Gaughan and Sullivan 2014).

There are several strategies that feedlot managers may utilise to limit the impact of high heat loads on feedlot cattle. Restricting feed or changing the timing of feed delivery may help reduce the body temperature of cattle under high heat loads. However, this is also likely to affect growth and should be used as a short-term strategy only (Mader et al. 2002). Allowing animals to acclimate to the local climate may also help animals cope better with temperatures outside their thermoneutral zone (Young and Hall 1993). Traditionally, managing heat load in feedlots has emphasised the importance of providing shade to feedlot cattle. Numerous studies have identified that feedlot cattle will utilise shade where available (Clarke and Kelly 1996; Gaughan et al. 2004b; Brown-Brandl et al. 2005; Eigenberg et al. 2010; Sullivan et al. 2011). Lees et al. (2020) showed that as heat-load intensity increased, there was a corresponding increase in shade utilisation by both Bos taurus and Bos indicus steers. Mitlöhner et al. (2001) and Castaneda et al. (2004) indicated that unshaded cattle seek shade by placing their heads in the shade cast by feed bunks during the hot hours of the day. Similarly, Gaughan and Mader (2014) and Lees et al. (2020) described unshaded cattle utilising the shade of other animals, water troughs and fence posts. Numerous studies have highlighted that unshaded cattle have higher respiration rates (Mitlöhner et al. 2002; Gaughan et al. 2004b; Brown-Brandl et al. 2005) and body temperatures (Clarke and Kelly 1996; Gaughan et al. 2010; Mader et al. 2010; Lees et al. 2018b) than do their shaded counterparts. However, there is some conjecture in the literature regarding the amount of shade (m2/animal) required to balance the impact of heat load (Clarke and Kelly 1996; Mitlöhner et al. 2002; Sullivan et al. 2011). Although shade structures can be beneficial for feedlot cattle under hot conditions, the reduction in solar radiation may be offset by a lack of air movement under the structure itself (Gaughan et al. 2004b). The design and functionality of the shade structure, as well as local climatic conditions, are, therefore, important considerations to enable adequate mitigation of high heat loads. Regardless, it is well established that shade availability is beneficial for feedlot cattle and an additional advantage is that the application is passive and reliant on voluntary use (Eigenberg et al. 2005).

Wetting cattle is another strategy that feedlot managers may employ to manage high heat loads. Studies utilising overhead water application for cooling feedlot cattle have predominantly focussed on reducing the impact of heat load on physiological responses such as increased body temperature, respiration rate and panting score (Davis et al. 2003; Gaughan et al. 2004a, 2008a; Mader et al. 2007; Brown-Brandl et al. 2010; Tresoldi et al. 2018). Morrison et al. (1973) found positive effects of sprinklers on feed intake and weight gain, but not feed efficiency in feedlot cattle. Cattle in the study by Morrison et al. (1973) were housed in a desert climate, on slatted floors, to avoid the issues of muddy pens developing as a result of the wetting process. Similar benefits were not found by Mitlöhner et al. 2001, who used misters on cattle housed in pens with partially slatted floors. The authors speculated that the finer droplet size generated by the misters may not have been as effective at cooling as are the larger droplets from sprinklers. Minimal production benefits have also been found when the pen surface rather than the cattle was wetted (Mader and Davis 2004). There is limited information regarding behavioural responses to sprinklers and their voluntary use. Mader et al. (2007) described feedlot cattle voluntarily using wetted areas during hot climatic conditions. More recently, Parola et al. (2012) concluded that beef cattle will voluntarily utilise overhead sprinklers during times of high heat load.

No studies have investigated the preference of cattle for sprinklers versus shade within feedlots. However, Schütz et al. (2011) described that dairy cattle prefer to use shade rather than sprinklers during summer. Utilising sprinklers to mitigate heat load has the potential to become a beneficial management tool (Mader et al. 2007), but there are some issues. At the cessation of wetting, cattle become suddenly exposed to hot conditions, to which they have not initiated physiological coping mechanisms, resulting in a rapid accumulation of heat (Gaughan et al. 2004a). Additionally, there is the potential for the application of water to increase humidity within the pen, reducing the efficiency of thermal-exchange mechanisms (Mader et al. 2007). Use of sprinklers may, therefore, be best suited to dry environments with limited humidity. Availability and access to adequate water in the Australian production environment may also prove challenging.

Animal comfort and productivity can also be compromised during exposure to cold, wet or windy conditions, as there is an increase in maintenance energy requirements (Belasco et al. 2015; Mader and Griffin 2015). Rainfall events have the potential to influence an animal’s thermal-exchange mechanisms, where a wet hide reduces the insulation properties of the coat (Vandenheede et al. 1995). Grandin (2016) suggested that managing wet and muddy conditions becomes difficult when annual rainfall exceeds 500 mm per annum.

Housing

The conditions in which cattle are housed influence their comfort and welfare. Cattle are highly motivated to lie down, generally resting for 4 to 10 h a day (Kilgour 2012), and a reduction in lying time may lead to chronic stress (Fisher et al. 2002). Studies have shown that dairy cattle will conduct operant tasks or push heavy weights for lying-time rewards (Tucker et al. 2018). Dairy cattle also prioritise lying over eating when restricted from both resources or given limited time to utilise both resources (Munksgaard et al. 2005). A review of cattle preferences showed that dairy cattle prefer to lie on soft, dry surfaces (Schütz et al. 2018). Similarly, Wilson et al. (2005) found that the pattern of lying and feeding in feedlot cattle changed depending on pen conditions and stocking density. Uncomfortable surface conditions, such as mud, may, therefore, pose a welfare risk to feedlot cattle (Grandin 2016). Cattle housed in muddy pens also have a tendency to reduce feeding frequencies, and muddy coats can compromise thermoregulation (Mader 2011). However, a study by Lee et al. (2016a) found that under warm weather conditions, increasing mud depth did not influence beef cattle preference for a feedlot environment compared with pasture. Standing in mud may also cause breaches to the skin barrier, increasing the risk of infectious diseases such as digital dermatitis and foot rot and, therefore, increasing the incidence of lameness (Davis-Unger et al. 2019). The incidence of muddy pens is likely to be influenced by several factors, including bedding availability, climate, pen-surface conditions, pen maintenance, and overall pen design, including the slope of the pen.

The National Guidelines for Beef Cattle Feedlots in Australia (2012) indicate a minimum space allowance for feedlot cattle of 9 m2/animal in open-air feedlots, a pen slope of 2–6% away from feed bunks and a pen-surface permeability of less than 1 × 10–9 m/s (National Guidelines for Beef Cattle Feedlots in Australia 2012). In the United States, mounds have been considered a useful addition to feedlot pens as they reduce mud problems during wet periods and can enhance airflow during hot conditions (Mader 2003). Mounds should be positioned perpendicular to the feedbunks, allowing water to easily run towards the drainage areas (Grandin 2016).

The negative impacts of muddy pens can be further negated by utilising bedding materials to absorb excess moisture from pen surfaces (Mader 2011; Grandin 2016). Several USA beef reports have outlined improvements in feed efficiency in open-air feedlot cattle provided with straw bedding under cold weather conditions (Birkelo and Lounsbery 1992; Mader and Colgan 2007). As feed intake did not increase, these gains may have been due to reduced maintenance-energy requirements. This suggests a potential for improved thermal comfort in these animals, although animal comfort and welfare were not directly measured in any study. In addition to straw, an Australian study compared other possible bedding materials, including woodchip, wood mulch, sawdust, timber off-cuts and building and construction waste. The suitability and effectiveness of these as bedding materials varies, as does their availability and economic viability (Watts et al. 2015). The potential for these beddings to contain sharp pieces or contaminants that may cause injury also needs to be considered. Good maintenance of pen surfaces may also reduce muddy conditions by allowing the surface to repel surplus water (Grandin 2016).

Feed and water

On entry to feedlots, cattle are typically transitioned from a roughage-based or grazing diet to a concentrate ration within a short period of time (<30 days). If not properly managed, this transition and the nature of the diet provided, can predispose animals to metabolic disorders such as acidosis (Galyean and Rivera 2003). From a survey of Australian feedlot producers, gastrointestinal-tract disorders were estimated to account for 8.6% of mortalities. The number of deaths peaked at about the third to fourth week on feed, before declining (Perkins 2013). Susceptibility to acidosis may vary among individuals, and management should be tailored towards the most susceptible animals (Galyean and Rivera 2003; Bevans et al. 2005).

Feedlot rations are typically developed in conjunction with nutritionists to maximise growth, while ensuring that the basic biological requirements of cattle are met and the risk of metabolic disorders is reduced. Optimal-diet theory (reviewed by Sih and Christensen 2001) suggests that foragers will prefer to consume feedstuffs that provide the greatest energy for the least amount of work and there is evidence that cattle will preferentially consume the most palatable portion of the ration (reviewed by Schütz et al. 2018). This is supported by Lee et al. (2013) who found that when given free access to both feedlot and pasture environments, cattle chose to obtain most of their daily nutrition requirements from the concentrate ration offered in the feedlot. Further, when a cost was added to their choice, cattle preferred a feedlot environment over pasture when feeding coincided with making a choice (Lee et al. 2013). However, when the feedlot diet was available ad libitum and a cost was added, no clear preferences were shown due to individual differences among cattle (Lee et al. 2016a).

Evidence also exists to suggest that cattle may shift their preferences to suit their physiological state. For example, cattle experiencing bouts of ruminal acidosis have been shown to shift their preference to favour the fibre component of the diet (DeVries et al. 2014). Van Os et al. (2017a) found that, in a motivation test, beef cattle on a high-grain feedlot diet would push nearly half their bodyweight to gain access to hay. They also found that cattle fed a hay diet would push similar weights to access additional hay, albeit with less urgency. In a similar study, Van Os et al. (2017b) found that there was no difference in the motivation of cattle fed hay to access a concentrate ration compared with accessing additional hay. While preference studies have limitations (see section Measures of preference and motivation to express behaviours and access resources), these findings raise interesting questions around an animal’s desire to satisfy its nutritional requirements in the easiest way possible versus the ability to search for different feeds (foraging). Additional research into the motivation of cattle to access additional concentrates if already fed a concentrate diet will help identify the value of the grazing and foraging processes, if any, as distinct from the satisfaction of basic nutritional requirements.

The use of in-feed antimicrobials may also have an impact on the welfare of feedlot cattle. While use of antimicrobials generally improves the health and, therefore, welfare of treated cattle, there is a risk that over-use will lead to development of resistance, reducing the efficacy of this treatment into the future (Badger et al. 2020). A survey of Australian feedlots suggested that in-feed antimicrobial use is not widespread and that antimicrobials are, generally, used strategically in at-risk pens to prevent gastrointestinal disorders (Badger et al. 2020).

Quantity as well as quality of water is another important welfare consideration in feedlots. Daily water intake is influenced by several bodily functions, including, but not limited to, regulation of core body temperature, growth, digestion and metabolism, and hydrolysis of proteins, fats and carbohydrates (National Research Council 2000; Arias and Mader 2011). However, daily water intake appears to be primarily driven by dry-matter intake, including the level of intake (kg/day) and the type of ration offered, i.e. concentrates versus roughage (McDowell and Weldy 1967). Feeding protocols may also play a role, with Ahlberg et al. (2018) finding that cattle fed ad libitum drank significantly more than did those that were not. Assessment of water requirements for cattle should consider both daily requirements and total annual requirement, flow rates needed for peak or short-term demand and design factors to prevent temperature build-up (Animal Health Australia 2014). There appears to be very little research on optimal space allowances at the water trough for feedlot cattle. However, even if sufficient space is provided, an additional welfare risk may occur in hot weather if cattle stand over the trough to cool themselves, reducing access for subordinate cattle (Castaneda et al. 2004).

For feedlots, it is important to ensure that appropriate water supplies can be met at all times of the year. Arias and Mader (2011) reported that feedlot cattle in the United States consumed 87.3% more water in summer than in winter (32.4 L/day versus 17.3 L/day, P < 0.01). The temperature of drinking water is an important factor influencing consumption; however, there is considerable variation within the literature as to the optimum temperature in relation to ambient conditions (Schütz et al. 2018). Regular cleaning of water troughs is also essential (Animal Health Australia 2014), especially where troughs are shared among pens, as this presents an increased disease-transfer risk (Hay et al. 2016).

The Australian Animal Welfare Standards and Guidelines for Cattle indicate that where water quality is known to be variable, water should be regularly monitored for harmful substances and managed so as to protect cattle welfare (Animal Health Australia 2014). The presence of salt, sulfates and faecal contamination may all result in reduced water intake and, subsequently, reduced feed consumption (Schütz et al. 2018).

Pregnancy management

The intake of pregnant females into feedlots is undesirable from both an economic and management perspective (Rademacher et al. 2015). Many cows will attempt to seek solitude and shelter when calving (Herring 2014), a process which may be hampered by the close confines of the feedlot environment. Frustration of this behavioural drive may cause additional stress on top of the calving process and represents a welfare concern for feedlot females. Other welfare concerns associated with feedlot pregnancies include calving dystocia, which may require a Caesarean, increased risk of mortality, as well as abortifacient induced morbidity (Buhman et al. 2003). Research from the United States regarding feedlot pregnancy management has generally focused on the identification of pregnant females and use of abortifacients (Buhman et al. 2003; Terrell et al. 2011; Rademacher et al. 2015). In Australia, the best-practice management guide for pregnant heifers (Bergman 2019) recommends a combination of strategies to reduce the risk of pregnant females calving on feed. Mainly, to reduce the number of pregnant females entering the feedlot by only purchasing steers or pregnancy-tested empty females, to determine the pregnancy status of high-risk females at arrival and to manage appropriately, and, finally, to monitor all females on feed for signs of impending parturition and remove them from the pen as soon as possible. Welfare of both the mother and calf need to be considered and managed in this situation. Further research into the incidence of this issue in Australia and the success of various management strategies is required to safeguard the welfare of pregnant females in the feedlot.

Psychological factors

Social interactions

Cattle allocations to pens are a reflection of the management style within each feedlot. Cattle are allocated to pen cohorts at induction and cattle numbers within pens are based on pen size and design. In Australia, this typically ranges from 50 to 200 head (Watts et al. 2016). Group housing allows the choice of social partners and the expression of social behaviour to occur (Gutmann et al. 2015b). However, social stress may ensue, as there is a disruption to the animals’ known social bonds, intermixing with new individuals and, ultimately, the establishment of a new social hierarchy (González et al. 2008). Agonistic behaviours often reduce as cattle become more familiar with their cohort and surroundings (McPhee et al. 1964; Tennessen et al. 1985), so the welfare risks associated with poor social interactions are predominantly concentrated at the beginning of the feedlot experience or each time animals are introduced into new groups. The physical implications of agonistic interactions may include injury and bruising and there is some evidence that the physiological stress of agonistic interactions may compound over time, particularly in subordinate animals (Mench et al. 1990). After initial mixing, pen cohorts typically remain together until the completion of the feeding period, unless required to be moved to a hospital or treatment pen. However, there are some instances where cattle that have not achieved market specifications remain at the feedlot and are regrouped until market specifications have been satisfied. Regrouping requires new dominance relationships to be established, resulting in an increase in agonistic interactions (Tennessen et al. 1985; Mounier et al. 2006; Jensen 2018). Regrouping within 2 weeks of slaughter may also have an impact on meat quality (Colditz et al. 2007). Boyland et al. (2016) suggested that, within a group of dairy cattle, keystone individuals may be particularly influential in maintaining the social structure. Therefore, understanding the social interactions among cattle within pen cohorts may also be an important consideration for feedlots. Abnormal social interactions such as Buller syndrome (repeated mounting of one steer by other steers; Brower and Kiracofe 1978; Edwards 1995) are an additional welfare risk above and beyond that associated with the normal establishment of social hierarchy and may lead to poor performance and increased risk of injury and death (Blackshaw et al. 1997). Steers subjected to repeated mounting may become exhausted and show evidence of trauma such as hair loss or swelling on the tail and rump (Tucker et al. 2015). The number of steers in a pen (but not stocking density), use of hormonal implants and method of mixing on feedlot entry have all been found to influence the incidence of Buller syndrome (Irwin et al. 1979).

Human–animal interactions

Humans are directly responsible for the emphasis placed on animal welfare and the living conditions provided to the animals within their care (Hansson and Lagerkvist 2014). Within the feedlot industry, stockpersons are directly responsible for the provision of food and water, maintaining health, identifying welfare issues and pen cleaning and maintenance. Therefore, there are numerous human–animal interactions that occur within a feedlot and it is important that stockpersons have sufficient training and experience in low-stress stock-handling techniques, as well as appropriate attitudes towards animals.

A stockperson’s behaviour towards animals and their welfare is largely driven by the attitudes and beliefs that individuals and teams hold (Hemsworth et al. 1993). Human personality attributes, including self-esteem and job satisfaction, can also influence a stockperson’s behaviour towards animals and welfare (Waiblinger et al. 2002; Boivin et al. 2003). In many instances, stockpersons do not have formal training and their attitudes are developed from observations of other stockpersons and personal experience (Hemsworth et al. 1993).

Numerous studies in other intensively managed species such as pigs and poultry have shown links between high levels of fear towards humans, stress and reduced productivity (reviewed by Hemsworth 2003). The use of cognitive-behavioural interventions with livestock handlers have also shown to improve both stockperson attitudes, animal behaviour and other job-related attributes, including staff-retention rates (Coleman et al. 2000). Research into the effects of human–animal interactions in beef cattle is less common, although a recent study confirmed that formal cattle-handling training has a positive outcome on stockpersons’ attitudes, consistency of handling and the incidence of undesirable animal behaviour (Ceballos et al. 2018).

Affective state

Affective state is a concept used to describe the emotions or feelings of an animal and can incorporate both positive (pleasant) and negative (unpleasant) experiences (Mendl et al. 2010; Mellor and Beausoleil 2015). Dismissed for a long time as anthropomorphic, there is now increasing evidence that the brain structure, physiology and behaviour of animals are similar enough to those of humans to produce comparable emotional experiences (Boissy et al. 2007; Hemsworth et al. 2015). Understanding the affective state of cattle within the feedlot environment, and the factors that influence it, will contribute towards understanding how cattle perceive the feedlot and whether the experience is positive or negative. Negative affective experiences include emotions such as thirst, hunger, nausea, breathlessness, pain, fear and anxiety. Positive affective experiences include emotions such as satiety, contentment, companionship, curiosity, playfulness, anticipatory joy, sexual pleasure and exploration (Hemsworth et al. 2015). It has now been recognised that good welfare needs to be extended to include the presence of positive experiences and emotions (Boissy et al. 2007).

There is a perception that housing cattle in feedlots may negatively influence affective states due to the inability to express natural levels of activity, such as would occur during grazing, potentially resulting in ‘boredom’ (Fureix et al. 2015). However, this may not be an issue if the diets have adequate fibre to promote rumination and lying time. There has been limited research to identify which resources are important to feedlot cattle and the behavioural needs required by cattle to satisfy their emotional state. It could be anticipated that cattle desire access to pasture solely to satisfy the need to graze, a behaviour that may be restricted by the feedlot environment if insufficient roughage is provided (Rutter 2010). However, when given a choice between a pasture and feedlot environment, cattle showed a preference for the pasture during the night where they spent 90% of the time lying down, and preferred the feedlot during the day where they consumed the majority of their daily nutritional requirements regardless of the quantity of pasture on offer (Lee et al. 2013). Similarly, Lee et al. (2013) found that while cattle generally preferred pasture when given free choice, they would still regularly choose to enter the feedlot environment even when there was a cost associated with their choice (being confined to the feedlot for between 8 and 16 h; Lee et al. 2016a). The authors also indicated that, in warmer temperatures, the choice to enter the feedlot continued regardless of mud depth (Lee et al. 2016a). These findings suggest that cattle may view the feedlot environment as a good place to find high-quality feed but as a less comfortable place for resting. This is supported by similar research in dairy cows showing preferences for pasture access at night (Charlton et al. 2013; Von Keyserlingk et al. 2017). Finally, Lee et al. (2016a) found that there was considerable individual variation in cattle preference towards the feedlot or pasture environment, suggesting that an underlying trait may influence an animal’s affective experience of the feedlot environment.

The inability to express highly motivated behaviours can lead to a high level of frustration. Stereotypies are maladaptive behaviours, often repetitive and periodic in nature, which are considered to arise from prolonged frustration (Williams and Randle 2017). Their role appears to be to reduce arousal in high-arousal situations. This is supported by the lowering of heart rates in beef calves performing stereotypies (Seo et al. 1998) and the complete suppression of stereotypies in adult beef breeder cows when a dopamine receptor antagonist is given, which eliminates the brain’s positive feedback mechanism in response to the behaviour (Sato et al. 1994). In non-tethered feedlot cattle, it has been suggested that oral behaviours such as tongue-playing are demonstrated as an alternative oral behaviour when time spent feeding is reduced, rather than a true stereotypy (Ishiwata et al. 2008; Daigle et al. 2017). Further research is required to identify the incidence and implications of oral stereotypies in Australian feedlot cattle.

Temperament and coping style

Cattle vary in their behavioural response to stressful events and this is broadly defined as temperament (MacKay and Haskell 2015; Finkemeier et al. 2018). The behavioural response to a real or perceived threat is the first step in the stress response of an animal (Moberg 2000). Although these behavioural responses can be a biologically advantageous reaction to danger, extreme or reactive responses may lead to chronic stress and negatively affect welfare, productivity and stockperson safety (Rushen et al. 1999). Behavioural responses can be complex and are a combination of inherent and environmental factors (Grandin 1997). A major cause of the complexity in behavioural response is variation in the perception of a stressor by individual animals (Mason and Mendl 1993; Moberg 2000). There is also evidence in many species that individuals may consistently adopt either a passive (reactive) or active (proactive) coping style when dealing with fear-evoking stressors (Koolhaas et al. 1999), and there is some indication that variation in coping styles also exists in cattle (Van Reenen et al. 2005).

Training programs that include positive handling for young cattle reared in extensive production systems can result in the cattle being easier to handle in later life (Fordyce et al. 1985), having reduced fearfulness of humans (Petherick et al. 2009), and improved productivity within intensive systems (Walker et al. 2007). However, substantial variation in temperament remains, even with good handling practices (Burrow and Dillon 1997; Walker et al. 2007; Petherick et al. 2009). The welfare of cattle in the feedlot environment and how they experience stress may, therefore, be influenced by the animal’s inherent temperament. The purchase of cattle with calmer or more appropriate temperaments is one method that may be utilised by feedlot management to improve the welfare and safety of both cattle and people. Evidence suggests that cattle with calmer temperaments can have improved productivity. However, the effects of temperament on economically important traits can be variable, and the biological basis for the effects is not fully understood (Ferguson et al. 2006; Kadel et al. 2006).


Established welfare measures

Improvements in animal welfare need to be based on managing measurable outcomes, allowing for the identification of risks and the implementation of effective management strategies (Lyles and Calvo-Lorenzo 2014). Identifying the success, or otherwise, of management strategies relies on sensitive, specific and repeatable measures of animal, resource and management factors that influence animal welfare. Therefore, understanding the role of quantitative data in identifying and improving animal management on farms has become a focal area in animal-welfare research (Sumner et al. 2018). There are several measures that already exist worldwide, or that could be practical and achievable measures of animal welfare within the Australian feedlot industry.

Measures of health

Mortality can be considered an indicator of animal-welfare standards across animal agricultural industries (Ring et al. 2018). To date, information pertaining to mortality and morbidity has been largely overlooked as a measure of welfare (Colditz et al. 2014; Thomsen and Houe 2018). Mortalities should be considered as the worst-case outcome in regards to welfare management (Colditz et al. 2014). However, it is extraordinarily rare to find a livestock enterprise where mortalities do not occur. Therefore, while mortalities occur within all facets of animal agriculture, the cause and prevention of mortalities becomes an important factor for welfare assessment. Furthermore, information pertaining to the number of animals found dead versus the number euthanised may provide a valuable indicator of monitoring and intervention protocols (Colditz et al. 2014). The method of euthanasia used may also become an important welfare measure. The Australian Animal Welfare Standards and Guidelines for Cattle recommend the use of a close-range firearm or a captive bolt, to the brain, for the humane killing of adult cattle and calves (Animal Health Australia 2014).

Although blunt measures of animal welfare, data describing the cause and number of morbidities, treatments, and hospital-pen management may provide an understanding of the welfare performance of a livestock enterprise (Colditz et al. 2014; Fox 2015; Vogel et al. 2015). An understanding of the effect of days on feed and seasonal influence on the prevalence and incidence of morbidities may also assist producers in tailoring management practices accordingly (Perkins 2013).

Measures of thermal comfort

Changes in body temperature are considered to be a reliable indicator of thermal status; however, they can be difficult to measure in commercial environments (Mader et al. 2010; Gaughan and Mader 2014). Under field conditions, the assessment of panting score (Table 1) has been considered a viable alternative to measuring body temperature, to assess the heat-load status of cattle (Brown-Brandl et al. 2006b; Mader et al. 2006; Gaughan et al. 2008b; Gaughan and Mader 2014). Panting score provides a visual assessment of respiratory dynamics in cattle and assesses the breathing or panting condition that the animal is displaying (Young and Hall 1993). The presence of shade-seeking behaviour (if possible) may also be indicative of cattle that are uncomfortable with their thermal environment.


Table 1.  Assessment of panting score, including breathing condition and associated respiration rate (RR, breaths per minute)
Adapted from Brown-Brandl et al. (2006a), Mader et al. (2006) and Gaughan et al. (2008b)
Click to zoom

In addition to single measures of thermal comfort, indices combining multiple climatic factors provide a prediction of the impact of extreme climatic conditions on welfare and production. The heat-load index and the accumulated heat-load model were developed for feedlot cattle and can be adjusted for varying genotypic and management factors (Gaughan et al. 2008b). Further development of these models has led to a Cattle Heat Load Toolbox2, which provides Australian feedlot producers with a 5-day heat-load forecasting service. Use of these tools not only allows feedlots to predict thermal comfort, but also to manage potential welfare risks associated with climatic extremes.

Resource measures

Measuring resources is generally a simple record of the quantity of a resource available or of the type or quality of the resource available. Determining the relevance of these resources to welfare and the required quantity for a positive welfare outcome is likely to be more difficult. For example, simple provision of water may not be adequate if the quality, temperature, flow rate and trough space per head is not suitable. Many of these factors will be highly specific to the feedlot and its location and will, therefore, need to be customised for individual feedlots.

Measures of human–animal interactions

The quality of animal handling can be assessed by the US Beef Quality Assurance Feedyard Assessment model, which includes measurements of rate of electric prod use, improper catches in the crush, number of cattle vocalising following restraint in the crush but before procedure, cattle stumbling when exiting crush, cattle falling on exiting the crush, and number of cattle jumping or running when exiting the crush (Table 2; Beef Quality Assurance 2017). The advantage of these assessments is that the response is a yes or no, as they either do or do not occur during handling, making them easy to measure at the individual level. These individual scores can then be compiled to provide an overall incidence for the group, with thresholds set for acceptable occurrences of each measure (Beef Quality Assurance 2017).


Table 2.  Assessment of cattle behaviour during handling
Adapted from Beef Quality Assurance (2017), Simon et al. (2016) and Woiwode et al. (2016)
Click to zoom

Numerous questionnaires and personality tests have been used to attempt to qualify stockperson attitudes, although tailoring of the questions to the animal species of interest is generally necessary (Windschnurer et al. 2009). Avoidance distance at the feed bunk has also been suggested as a possible indicator of the human–animal relationship that has shown good inter-observer reliability (Windschnurer et al. 2009) and that does not appear to be influenced by the dominance or flightiness of other animals in the pen (Mazurek et al. 2011). The limitations of this and other tests designed to assess the human–animal relationship have been critically reviewed by Waiblinger et al. (2006).

Measures of demeanour

An animal’s demeanour or body language may provide important information about its physical and mental state. Qualitative Behavioural Assessment (QBA) is a technique used to identify subtle differences in behavioural expression that can then be converted to a numerical score. QBA forms part of the suite of welfare assessments for livestock administered in Europe by Welfare Quality (Welfare Quality 2009), and, in research, it has predominantly been used to assess the behaviour of beef cattle during transport (Stockman et al. 2011, 2013) and at slaughter (Stockman et al. 2012). Advantages of QBA are that they do not require specialised equipment and require limited training to conduct an assessment. Fleming et al. (2016) reviewed the use of QBA for assessing animal welfare, addressing key issues including reliability, sensitivity, versatility and feasibility. The resulting scores have been shown to have strong correlations with physiological indicators of stress, other quantitative measures of behaviour (Fleming et al. 2016), and measures of temperament (Sant’Anna and Paranhos da Costa 2013; Góis et al. 2016). Limitations to the technique have also been identified. For instance, observers shown footage of nervous animals in conjunction with the test footage subsequently scored the test-footage animals as more nervous (Fleming et al. 2015). Despite this difference, the overall ranking of animals did not change (Fleming et al. 2015). Furthermore, the time of day that video clips are scored may influence the outcome of QBA assessment (Gutmann et al. 2015a). Finally, the interpretation of QBA scores as an indicator of overall animal welfare requires experienced evaluation. Therefore, it has been suggested that QBA should be considered with other welfare assessments, rather than as a stand-alone measure of animal welfare (Fleming et al. 2016).

Measures of temperament and coping style

Numerous methods can be utilised to evaluate temperament using the escape and avoidance behaviours that cattle display when responding to stressors such as handling by humans, exposure to novel objects or social isolation (reviewed by Burrow 1997). These tests range from very simple objective or subjective measures to complex behavioural tests and assess various aspects of cattle behaviour in restrained and non-restrained situations. Wearable-sensor technology may also provide data on behavioural attributes such as activity levels in the home pen, which could be indicative of temperament or underlying coping styles (MacKay et al. 2013).

Two tests that are simple and safe to measure, are moderately to highly heritable (Burrow 1997; Kadel et al. 2006) and are currently being used by the beef cattle industry to select for calmer temperaments are flight speed (Burrow et al. 1988) and crush score (Grandin 1993). Flight speed is the electronically recorded speed at which an animal exits the crush when released and is an objective measure of temperament. Crush score is a subjective assessment of the level of agitation an animal displays when confined in the crush (Table 3). Slower flight speeds and lower crush scores are associated with calmer temperaments. Temperament is a complex trait and the specific aspect of temperament being measured by these tests is a continuing field of research (Burrow 1997; Petherick et al. 2002, 2009; Kilgour et al. 2006; Müller and von Keyerslingk 2006; Lee et al. 2018). Regardless, higher scores on both crush score and flight speed have been associated with reduced productivity (Burrow and Dillon 1997; Voisinet et al. 1997a, 1997b; Petherick et al. 2002, 2009; Cafe et al. 2011a), lower meat quality (Fordyce et al. 1988; Voisinet et al. 1997a; King et al. 2006; Cafe et al. 2011a), increased stress responses (Curley et al. 2008; Cafe et al. 2011b), and reduced immune function (Fell et al. 1999; Hine et al. 2019).


Table 3.  Assessment of crush score in cattle
Adapted from Grandin (1993), Cafe et al. (2011a) and Lee et al. (2018)
Click to zoom


Proxy indicators of welfare

Proxy indicators of animal welfare are indirect measures that can imply or be representative of improved or reduced welfare. These are usually used when a direct measure is not available, or when the cost, complexity or timeliness of data collection make the use of a direct measure impractical.

Production

Identifying production traits that can be utilised as direct indicators of cattle welfare may be difficult. Liveweights of animals, when entering the feedlot, are likely to be influenced by the type of cattle (breed and sex), point of origin (geographical and sale yard versus direct from producer), season, environmental conditions and target market specifications (domestic versus export markets). Production performance is also likely to have some variability across feedlot enterprises, which may be associated with differences in diet composition or diet ingredients as well as the genetic potential of cattle (Sowell et al. 1997; Gardner et al. 1999; Brown-Brandl et al. 2006a; Belasco et al. 2015). However, clinical and subclinical disease and other environmental stressors may influence the allocation of nutrients to production traits (Gardner et al. 1999; Galyean and Rivera 2003; Colditz 2004; Sackett et al. 2006). This has generated interest in using production data as a proxy indicator of welfare by representing how well individual animals are coping with their environment (Colditz and Hine 2016; Berghof et al. 2019). The capacity of an animal to maintain a production trajectory over time, such as growth rate or milk production, in the face of day-to-day fluctuations in environmental conditions, is termed resilience (Colditz and Hine 2016; Scheffer et al. 2018). Resilience of production traits provides an integrated measure of the impacts of the psychological and physical environment on the physiology of the animal (Colditz and Hine 2016; Scheffer et al. 2018).

Continuous electronic monitoring of livestock is increasing the availability of data suitable for estimating resilience. The main focus has been on analysing variables such as immune responsiveness, feed intake and milk yield in beef cattle, sheep, pigs and dairy cattle for estimating heritability of resilience for use in breeding programs (Elgersma et al. 2018; Putz et al. 2019; Hine et al. 2019). As well as the use of genetic selection to improve the resilience of progeny, it is recognised that resilience also provides a contemporary measure of the welfare of individual animals (Colditz and Hine 2016; Berghof et al. 2019; Elgersma et al. 2018). Development of resilience measures based on production variables, behaviours (e.g. feed intake) or physiological measures (e.g. body temperature, immune function) may provide new proxy measures of welfare of feedlot cattle.

Environmental enrichment

In many production systems, providing what is perceived as a completely ‘natural’ environment is not possible or may not result in improved welfare, for example, by increasing exposure to predators. Shortfalls in the ability to satisfy the behavioural needs of the animal may be fully or partially addressed using environmental enrichment. Environmental enrichment consists of providing animals with stimuli that promote the expression of normal, species-specific behavioural activities, potentially benefiting animal welfare through reduced stress and frustration (Ishiwata et al. 2006). The provision of environmental enrichment has been shown to improve behaviour, physiological responses and carcass characteristics for cattle (Wilson et al. 2002b; Ishiwata et al. 2006) and sheep (Aguayo-Ulloa et al. 2014) in feedlot environments. Ishiwata et al. (2006) found that average daily gain and marbling score were improved in cattle provided with a grooming and foraging device. Similarly, Aguayo-Ulloa et al. (2014) found that lambs given access to a feeding platform, straw forage and a ramp for play, had higher average daily gains, heavier carcasses and higher fat scores. The authors also reported reduced stereotypies, improved performance on a cognitive task, greater levels of immunocompetence and lower muscle pH at slaughter in enriched lambs (Aguayo-Ulloa et al. 2014). Furthermore, piglets reared in socially enriched environments were more tolerant of unfamiliar pigs when mixed at the growing phase (Li and Wang 2011). Further studies are required to determine the optimal type of enrichment and the length of exposure to sufficiently enhance the feedlot environment. Wilson et al. (2002b) investigated the value of different enrichment devices to feedlot cattle, concluding that grooming devices were well utilised, while scent devices held little interest beyond Day 2. Provision of environmental enrichment could provide an indicator of improved welfare; however, care would need to be taken that the type of enrichment utilised is species appropriate and addresses specific behavioural needs and that provision of the enrichment does not generate additional social stress or aggression over the device.

Coat cleanliness

Lying time is an accurate and reliable measure of animal comfort; however, behavioural sampling is time consuming and labour intensive, with measurements being required at least every 30 min to get an accurate assessment of lying duration (Mitlöhner et al. 2001). An alternative proxy indicator of animal comfort, as well as good pen maintenance and design, may be coat cleanliness score (Table 4; Grandin 2016). While coat cleanliness shows promise as a proxy measure, further research is needed to identify the relationship between lying time and coat cleanliness under different surface conditions.


Table 4.  Assessment of coat-cleanliness score
Adapted from Grandin (2016)
Click to zoom

Positive social interactions

Some behaviours are inherently rewarding in the longer term, without having an apparent immediate benefit to the animal. These behaviours tend to occur when other needs are satisfied and can, therefore, potentially be used as proxy indicators that the animal’s primary needs are being met. Allogrooming and play are two such behaviours that may prove useful indicators of an overall positive affective state. Allogrooming in cattle has a role in the formation and maintenance of social bonds within social groups (Sato et al. 1993; Šárová et al. 2016). Although the presence of allogrooming suggests that, overall, the social harmony of the group will be improved in the long term, subordinate animals may find being groomed by a dominant animal stressful (Boissy et al. 2007). The immediate welfare benefits to the individual may vary; however, allogrooming could still be a useful indicator of improved welfare states within the group. Greater understanding of the relationship between time spent allogrooming and positive welfare outcomes is required. Durations vary considerably among studies, where allogrooming sessions have been reported to last between 1 and 814 s (Sato et al. 1991, 1993; Val-Laillet et al. 2009; Šárová et al. 2016).

Similarly, play behaviour may be a possible candidate for an animal-based measure of positive welfare (Winckler et al. 2003; Boissy et al. 2007; Held and Špinka 2011; Mellor 2015). The advantage of play behaviour as an indicator of positive emotions is that it can be easily interpreted by non-professional observers (Boissy et al. 2007). Furthermore, play behaviour incorporates elements of functional behaviour, including fleeing, fighting, sexual and predatory behaviours (Boissy et al. 2007). The use of allogrooming and play as indicators of positive welfare have so far been unsuccessful due to the intermittent occurrence of the behaviours and poor repeatability (Boissy et al. 2007; Knierim and Winckler 2009). Additionally, many of the studies on both allogrooming and play behaviour have focussed on dairy cattle; therefore, there is a need to quantify these behaviours in feedlot cattle.

Abnormal behaviours

The incidence of abnormal behaviours, such as stereotypies, may provide an indicator of reduced welfare within a feedlot. Oral stereotypies have been reported in dairy cattle (Redbo 1992) and, to a lesser extent, beef cattle (Sato et al. 1994), with approximately half of penned steers in one study showing some form of short-term (<20 min) tongue playing (Ishiwata et al. 2008). Daigle et al. (2017) found that the incidence of repeated animal–environment interactions such as tongue rolling, pica, navel or ear sucking increased the longer the weaned calves were confined in dry-lot pens, with ~15% of calves performing the behaviour by Week 5 of the observation period. Further research into the incidence of abnormal behaviours in feedlots and the implications for welfare is required.

Deviations from normal patterns of behaviour may also be indicative of health and welfare issues. Stockpersons who are responsible for monitoring cattle on a daily basis are best placed to identify deviations from normal behaviour; however, this is a subjective measure that relies on the skills of the stockperson and the effectiveness of any training programs. Several commercial and research options for wearable-sensor technology3 offer the possibility of remotely monitoring behaviours such as lying and ruminating, so as to identify deviations as early as possible (Rahman et al. 2018). While predominantly focussed on the dairy industry, the technology has the potential to be easily transferred to other intensive systems, including feedlots.


Future directions for measuring welfare

Several methods exist within the research setting to measure some of the more complex indicators of welfare. Some of these tests are still in the development stage requiring further validation, while others are established measures that are currently too impractical or expensive to use in a commercial feedlot environment. While currently unable to be used by industry, these measures remain of interest as they may be able to inform the development of industry appropriate tests, or alternatively, advances in technology may make these measures more practical to apply.

Measures of preference and motivation to express behaviours and access resources

To assess the motivation of an animal to perform a behaviour or access a resource, it is important to understand the inherent significance of that behaviour or resource to the animal. One method of determining the significance of a behaviour or resource to an animal is to conduct a single-choice test (preference testing). These tests are appealing for their simplicity; however, several limitations to single-choice tests have been identified (Kirkden and Pajor 2006). Internal and external factors may influence preference or motivation for a particular resource (Legrand et al. 2011; Parola et al. 2012; Chen et al. 2013, 2016). Alternatively, preference may be affected by prior experience, with familiar resources being typically preferred over novel ones. Finally, it can be difficult to interpret findings when animals fail to exclusively choose a single resource and instead use both for varying amounts of time (Lee et al. 2013).

Motivation tests avoid some of these limitations by assessing how hard an animal is willing to work for a particular resource. For example, animals can be trained to push a weighted gate to gain access to a resource and the amount of weight is then slowly increased. The percentage of bodyweight willing to be pushed to access a resource can then be taken as a measure of motivation. Alternatively, a cost may be associated with a choice, such as being limited to that resource for a period of time (Lee et al. 2016a). These methods may also have some limitations. Van Os et al. (2017b) found evidence of contrafreeloading in cattle where they were willing to work to access a resource that was simultaneously freely available to them. The authors speculated that the work may itself be rewarding, or allow the cattle to express control over their environment (Van Os et al. 2017b). These factors should be considered when assessing the importance of a resource using motivation tests. Finally, both preference or motivation tests tend to present limited options of choices, including, for example, a choice between two resources or the choice between working or not working to access a resource. The types of options provided are determined by the researcher and do not account for the possibility that an alternative resource may be of even greater value to the animal.

Measures of fearfulness

Fearfulness is a temperament trait indicative of an animal’s tendency to display excessive fear in potentially threatening situations (Forkman et al. 2007). While some aspects of overall temperament can be assessed easily (see Measures of temperament and coping style section), measurements of fear and fearfulness are less established. Tests of fearfulness generally rely on the application of a sudden, unfamiliar or unpredictable stimulus to elicit a fear response (reviewed by Forkman et al. 2007). The most commonly utilised tests involve measuring the behavioural response to a novel area or object (Forkman et al. 2007). Repeatability and reliability of fear tests have been found to be low in most studies (Forkman et al. 2007; Meagher et al. 2016). Several issues have been identified with these tests. First, that calm, disinterested or ill animals will show a similar delay in approaching or exploring a novel stimulus as a fearful one (Meagher et al. 2016). Second, that there is likely to be a high level of habituation to novel stimuli and, therefore, the repeatability of tests is low (Forkman et al. 2007). However, the speed of habituation or how quickly an animal acclimates to a situation may also be indicative of temperament and coping style, although measuring this at an individual level is difficult (Monk et al. 2018a). Finally, individual variation in cognitive test results can be influenced by several outside factors that need to be considered when analysing results (Bushby et al. 2018).

Handling tests are less frequently used and vary considerably in methodologies. Such tests generally involve the presence of a human in conjunction with a husbandry procedure, such as restraint or separation (Forkman et al. 2007). Although there has been some correlation with handling tests and physiological parameters (Forkman et al. 2007), measures of repeatability have varied among studies (Welp et al. 2004; Turner et al. 2011). The relevance of current tests of fearfulness to the feedlot environment also needs to be considered. Feedlot animals would very rarely experience the social isolation frequently incorporated into these tests. The welfare implications for an animal showing high fearfulness in these tests may not eventuate in an environment in which they have companions to socially buffer their experiences.

Measures of affective state

Measures of affective state need to include measures of both positive and negative emotional valence. Research into positive affective states continues to emerge; however, measures of negative affective state remain more common and considerably more work on positive affective state is required. The complexity of most of these tests makes them currently unsuitable for industry-based measures of affective state for feedlot cattle. However, their use in the research setting may prove valuable to identify proxy indicators or to identify the types of resources required by cattle to promote a positive affective state.

Cognitive bias

It is known in both humans and animals that affective state can influence cognitive functioning, in particular, generating specific cognitive biases in attention, memory and judgement (Boissy et al. 2007; Mendl et al. 2010). Therefore, there is potential to utilise these biases to detect and quantify the effect of the feedlot environment on the underlying affective state and the emotional implications of providing or limiting access to particular resources.

Judgement bias has been assessed in several farm animal species (reviewed by Baciadonna and McElligott 2015). The assessment of judgement bias is based on the principle that the underlying emotional state influences the likelihood of a subject interpreting an ambiguous cue in a pessimistic or optimistic manner. Judgement bias is typically assessed through a go/no-go task in which animals are trained to respond one-way to a positively associated cue and another way to a negatively associated cue. The animal’s response to an ambiguous cue is then assessed under the hypothesis that those in a negative affective state are more likely to interpret the ambiguous cue as predictive of the negative outcome. Negative judgement biases have been shown in dairy cattle in response to both physical (Neave et al. 2013) and psychological (Daros et al. 2014) stressors; however, only a few studies in other species have assessed positive judgement biases (Douglas et al. 2012; Verbeek et al. 2014). Disadvantages of this task are that it requires considerable training and may be confounded by low levels of motivation to complete one of the responses (Baciadonna and McElligott 2015). While judgement bias appears to be a valid measure of affective state, care must be taken to ensure that the emotional state of interest (positive or negative) is actually activated. Doyle et al. (2010) and Sanger et al. (2011) found that sheep exposed to an acute stressor, aimed at evoking a negative affective state, actually showed a positive judgement bias on release. It is also important for the validity of the results that animals are cognitively capable of distinguishing among the positive, negative and ambiguous cues, as well as distinguishing between the rewarding or punishing properties of a stimulus.

One of the main disadvantages of tests of judgement bias is the requirement for training. Tests of attention bias avoid this requirement, potentially providing a quicker and easier measure of an affective state (Monk et al. 2018b). Attention-bias tests are based on the idea that animals with a negative affective state will show greater attention towards threatening stimuli than do animals in a positive or neutral affective state (Lee et al. 2016b; Crump et al. 2018). In sheep, attention-bias paradigms have been well refined and pharmacologically validated (Monk et al. 2018b). A study has successfully extended the paradigm to cattle with similar pharmacological treatment responses (Lee et al. 2018). Recently, attention biases have been identified for depression, a longer-term negative affective state (Monk et al. 2018c). Further studies to determine whether attention bias can identify positive affective states are needed.

Lateralisation bias

There is evidence in humans and animals that emotional valence can be reflected in asymmetric brain activity that is determined by left or right hemisphere-dominant behaviour (Rogers 2010; Leliveld et al. 2013). It has been hypothesised that the right hemisphere is dominant in processing information when in a negative affective state and, therefore, results in an increased use of left visual and auditory fields and left motor responses (Rogers 2010; Leliveld et al. 2013). When presented with two bilaterally placed novel objects, dairy cattle that were more hesitant to approach the objects tended to explore the left object rather than the right one (Kappel et al. 2017). Similarly, when presented with a novel person walking through the herd, cattle tended to cross the person’s path so as to be able to assess the experimenter with their left monocular field significantly more often than with their right field, and this pattern was reversed once the experimenter became familiar (Robins and Phillips 2010).

The use of lateralised presentation of stimuli for management purposes may also serve to reduce stress, such as, for example, by handling animals from the appropriate side (Robins et al. 2018). However, further studies are required to confirm these findings (Leliveld et al. 2013). The identification of the correct side may also incite production benefits. Rizhova and Kokorina (2005) found that dairy cows repeatedly presented with food from the left side had improved reproductive success. There was also an effect on milk production, where cows under poor feeding conditions improved milk yield when food was presented from the right, and cows under good feeding conditions increased yield when food was presented from the left (Rizhova and Kokorina 2005). Findings from the limited number of studies investigating affective state and lateralisation suggest that considerable research is required to understand the true nature of this relationship.

Eye whites

The amount of visible eye white has been suggested as a measure of both positive and negative emotional states. Increases in visible eye white in dairy cows have been demonstrated in response to waiting for feed (Sandem et al. 2006), offering inedible feeds (Lambert and Carder 2017), and depriving access to visible feed (Sandem et al. 2002). Decreases in visible eye white have been observed in response to stroking (Proctor and Carder 2015), and the provision of food (Sandem et al. 2002, 2006; Lambert and Carder 2017). Visible eye white has also been positively correlated with other indicators of frustration, including aggression and vocalisation (Sandem et al. 2002). Although these findings appear reliable and repeatable, Gómez et al. (2018) found no difference in visible eye white between a positive feeding experience and a negative hoof-trimming experience. They also found that visible eye white differed among dairy breeds (Gómez et al. 2018). Issues also occur around determining whether eye whites increase in response to arousal or changes in emotional state. Lambert and Carder (2017) found that visible eye whites in dairy cows were highest while eating concentrates (a supposedly positive experience), compared with pre- and post-feeding. The use of eye white measures as an indicator of affective state requires further research to determine its applicability to beef cattle and the feedlot environment.

Stress physiology

Affective experiences are not only a reflection of external influences but can also be seen as a response to the physiological inputs generated by the animal’s internal state (Boissy et al. 2007; Hemsworth et al. 2015). Stress, pain and fear are examples of negative affective experiences resulting from physiological changes (Hemsworth et al. 2015). Physiology is a complex and detailed field and it is not the purpose of the present review to comprehensively describe every aspect of stress physiology. The focus herein is to provide an overview of the non-invasive physiological measures of affective state, which may, in the future, have practical applicability in the feedlot industry. Most physiological measures focus on the action of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis, known to be activated by stress, but which also regulates other biological processes such as reproduction and immune responses (Boissy et al. 2007). Increased HPA axis activity is known to be indicative of short-term stress; however, chronic stress may result in negative feedback regulation and HPA axis hypoactivity, and, so, careful interpretation of results is required.

Hair cortisol

Blood, urine and salivary cortisol concentrations are well established as measures of stress (Meyer and Novak 2012). However, the inherently stressful nature of sample collection, interference from circadian variation and short circulating half-life limit their applicability as a measure of chronic stress. Faecal cortisol metabolites can also be used to estimate stress in the previous 12–24 h, utilising less invasive sampling techniques. Limitations to faecal cortisol include potential modification of concentrations during gut transit and the need for samples to be frozen before testing (Tallo-Parra et al. 2015). Measurement of hair cortisol concentration has been suggested as an alternative measure of long-term cortisol accumulated over weeks and months (Meyer and Novak 2012). The mode of deposition of cortisol into the hair shaft is still unclear. However, it has been shown to be stable at room temperature for at least a year (Meyer and Novak 2012), and is relatively unaffected by environmental factors (Montillo et al. 2014). Moya et al. (2013) standardised a method of measuring hair cortisol concentration in cattle, which included clipping rather than plucking the hair and using hair from the tail switch to provide the best measure of cortisol concentration. Comin et al. (2013) found that hair cortisol differed between healthy dairy cows and those that had recently suffered a disease or that had been physiologically challenged by calving in the last month. Sensitivity and specificity of their nominated cut-off value was 62.4% and 69.3% respectively. Creutzinger et al. (2017) investigated hair cortisol and found a treatment effect of pain relief on surgically castrated beef cattle over 14 days, suggesting that hair cortisol may also be an appropriate measure of acute stress. Limitations include the possibility of diluting out the cortisol concentration by including portions of the hair shaft that grew before or after the stressor of interest. Clipping the area of interest to create a controlled baseline may avoid this issue.

Body temperature

In addition to the environmental factors affecting body temperature described in Thermal comfort section, changes to the regular circadian pattern of body temperature occur during infection and stress (Vinkers et al. 2009). During stress-induced hyperthermia, as seen for instance when cattle are subjected to an attention-bias test (Lee et al. 2018), activation of the sympathetic nervous system causes blood to be diverted away from the periphery and towards the vital organs in a fight–flight response (Bouwknecht et al. 2007; Proctor and Carder 2016). This causes a short-term increase in the core body temperature that can last as little as 20–30 min and a decrease in temperature in peripheral organs such as the eyes and nose. In contrast, cytokine-induced changes in body temperature (fever) during infection and immune activation can extend over days. Historically, measuring body temperature requires capture and handling to insert the temperature sensors into the tympanic membrane (Mader et al. 2010), abdominal cavity (Lefcourt and Adams 1996), rectum or vaginal cavity (Vickers et al. 2010; Lees et al. 2018a) or the rumen (Lees et al. 2018b). Methods of measuring peripheral temperature, such as infrared thermography, may provide a less invasive assessment of body temperature within the feedlot environment, although some limitations exist. Decreases in eye temperature have been found in response to aversive handling (Stewart et al. 2008), surgical castration (Stewart et al. 2010a) and epinephrine infusion (Stewart et al. 2010b). However, changes in eye temperature are potentially breed dependent (Gómez et al. 2018) and can be affected by environmental conditions (Church et al. 2014). Nasal temperature in dairy cows has been shown to decrease in response to both positive and negative emotional states (Proctor and Carder 2015; Proctor and Carder 2016), suggesting that changes in peripheral temperature may not just be a useful measure of stress but also of positive affective state. Further studies are required to quantify this relationship. Optimal conditions for imaging, including the avoidance of direct sunlight, high humidity, wind and temperature extremes may also be difficult to achieve in an outdoor feedlot environment (Okada et al. 2013).


Conclusions

Several factors may influence the welfare of cattle in the feedlot environment. These can become an issue when not properly monitored and managed. These include, but are not limited to, animal factors such as the inability of cattle to effectively respond to environmental extremes, inability to express their full range of natural behaviours such as grazing, and unsuitable temperaments, as well as management factors, such as stockperson skills in identifying morbidities, comfort of surface conditions for lying, stock-handling methods and yard design, identification and management of pregnancy and mixing of unfamiliar cattle. These issues, while significant, are not insurmountable, with several strategies already being employed within the feedlot industry to address them. Continuous improvements in these areas will require accurate, reliable and repeatable measures of welfare factors to allow quantification of current and future welfare states. Whereas measures of physical factors affecting welfare are well established, considerable work is still required to generate measures of psychological welfare that have practical applications in the feedlot industry.

It is important that future research focuses on developing a greater understanding of how the animal itself perceives the feedlot environment and which factors and resources are required to improve or maintain an overall positive affective state in animals confined to a feedlot. Research into the preferences and motivations of feedlot cattle is also needed, so as to identify which aspects from a pasture environment are required to satisfy the behavioural needs of cattle. The identification of these preferences will allow for the development of environmental enrichment and the provision of resources such as bedding and shade to ensure positive welfare outcomes for feedlot cattle. Further work assessing the affective state of cattle under various management conditions will clarify the effectiveness of any interventions in satisfying the psychological needs of feedlot cattle. Finally, a better understanding of the temperament types and coping styles that allow cattle to best adapt to the feedlot environment with minimal adverse physical and psychological consequences is required to maximise cattle welfare in feedlot enterprises. To help achieve these outcomes, and to monitor their success, it is also important that future research focuses on developing practical tools to accurately assess animal welfare in real time in a commercial feedlot setting.


Conflicts of interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.



Acknowledgements

The authors acknowledge Jim Rothwell, Amanda Doughty, Dougal Gordon and Meat & Livestock Australia Ltd for their comments and suggestions on improving this manuscript before submission. This project was supported by funding from Meat & Livestock Australia Ltd (North Sydney, NSW, Australia), the Commonwealth Scientific and Industrial Research Organisation (CSIRO, Canberra, ACT, Australia) and the NSW Department of Primary Industries (NSW DPI, NSW, Australia) through the Animal Welfare Strategic Partnership. Meat and Livestock Australia project number P.PSH.0807.


References

Aguayo-Ulloa LA, Villarroel M, Pascual-Alonso M, Miranda-de la Lama GC, María GA (2014) Finishing feedlot lambs in enriched pens using feeder ramps and straw and its influence on behavior and physiological welfare indicators. Journal of Veterinary Behavior: Clinical Applications and Research 9, 347–356.
Finishing feedlot lambs in enriched pens using feeder ramps and straw and its influence on behavior and physiological welfare indicators.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Ahlberg CM, Allwardt K, Broocks A, Bruno K, McPhillips L, Taylor A, Krehbiel CR, Calvo-Lorenzo MS, Richards CJ, Place SE, DeSilva U, VanOverbeke DL, Mateescu RG, Kuehn LA, Weaber RL, Bormann JM, Rolf MM (2018) Environmental effects on water intake and water intake prediction in growing beef cattle Journal of Animal Science 96, 4368–4384.
Environmental effects on water intake and water intake prediction in growing beef cattleCrossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 30169660PubMed |

Ames D (1980) Thermal environment affects production efficiency of livestock. Bioscience 30, 457–460.
Thermal environment affects production efficiency of livestock.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Animal Health Australia (2014) ‘Australian animal welfare standards and guidelines for cattle.’ Available at www.animalwelfarestandards.net.au.[Verified 1 March 2018]

Arias RA, Mader TL (2011) Environmental factors affecting daily water intake on cattle finished in feedlots. Journal of Animal Science 89, 245–251.
Environmental factors affecting daily water intake on cattle finished in feedlots.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 20870953PubMed |

Australian Lot Feeders Association (2015) ‘Frequently asked questions.’ Available at http://www.feedlots.com.au/faq.[Verified 22 February 2018]

Baciadonna, L, McElligott, AG (2015) ‘The use of judgement bias to assess welfare in farm livestock.’ (The Humane Society Institute for Science and Policy, Animal Studies Repository. Available at https://animalstudiesrepository.org/ [Verified 12 March 2018] )

Badger SM, Sullivan KF, Jordan D, Caraguel CG, Page SW, Cusack PM, Frith D, Trott DJ (2020) Antimicrobial use and stewardship practices on Australian beef feedlots. Australian Veterinary Journal 98, 37–47.
Antimicrobial use and stewardship practices on Australian beef feedlots.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 31721160PubMed |

Baumgard LH, Rhoads RP (2012) Ruminant nutrition symposium: ruminant production and metabolic responses to heat stress. Journal of Animal Science 90, 1855–1865.
Ruminant nutrition symposium: ruminant production and metabolic responses to heat stress.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 22205665PubMed |

Beef Quality Assurance (2017) ‘BQA feedyard assessment.’ Available at https://www.bqa.org.[Verified 2 March 2018]

Belasco EJ, Cheng Y, Schroeder TC (2015) The impact of extreme weather on cattle feeding profits. Journal of Agricultural and Resource Economics 40, 285–305.

Berghof T, Poppe M, Mulder H (2019) Opportunities to improve resilience in animal breeding programs. Frontiers in Genetics 9, 692
Opportunities to improve resilience in animal breeding programs.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 30693014PubMed |

Bergman E (2019) ‘Feedlot best practice management: pregnant heifers.’ (Meat & Livestock Australia: Sydney, NSW, Australia)

Bevans DW, Beauchemin KA, Schwartzkopf-Genswein KS, McKinnon JJ, McAllister TA (2005) Effect of rapid or gradual grain adaptation on subacute acidosis and feed intake by feedlot cattle. Journal of Animal Science 83, 1116–1132.
Effect of rapid or gradual grain adaptation on subacute acidosis and feed intake by feedlot cattle.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 15827257PubMed |

Birkelo CP, Lounsbery J (1992) Effect of straw and newspaper bedding on cold season feedlot performance in two housing systems. South Dakota Beef report. Paper 12. Available at http://openprairie.sdstate.edu/sd_beefreport_1992/12. [Verified 27 March 2019]

Bitman J, Lefcourt A, Wood DL, Stroud B (1984) Circadian and Ultradian Temperature Rhythms of Lactating Dairy Cows. Journal of Dairy Science 67, 1014–1023.
Circadian and Ultradian Temperature Rhythms of Lactating Dairy Cows.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 6736397PubMed |

Blackshaw J, Blackshaw A (1994) Heat stress in cattle and the effect of shade on production and behavior: a review. Australian Journal of Experimental Agriculture 34, 285–295.
Heat stress in cattle and the effect of shade on production and behavior: a review.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Blackshaw JK, Blackshaw AW, McGlone JJ (1997) Buller steer syndrome review. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 54, 97–108.
Buller steer syndrome review.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Boissy A, Manteuffel G, Jensen MB, Moe RO, Spruijt B, Keeling LJ, Winckler C, Forkman B, Dimitrov I, Langbein J, Bakken M, Veissier I, Aubert A (2007) Assessment of positive emotions in animals to improve their welfare. Physiology and Behavior 92, 375–397.
Assessment of positive emotions in animals to improve their welfare.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 17428510PubMed |

Boivin X, Lensink J, Tallet C, Veissier I (2003) Stockmanship and farm animal welfare. Animal Welfare 12, 479–492.

Bond TE, Kelly CF, Morrison SR, Periera N (1967) Solar, atmospheric, and terrestrial radiation received by shaded and unshaded animals. Transactions of the ASAE. American Society of Agricultural Engineers 10, 622–627.
Solar, atmospheric, and terrestrial radiation received by shaded and unshaded animals.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Bouwknecht JA, Olivier B, Paylor RE (2007) The stress-induced hyperthermia paradigm as a physiological animal model for anxiety: a review of pharmacological and genetic studies in the mouse. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 31, 41–59.
The stress-induced hyperthermia paradigm as a physiological animal model for anxiety: a review of pharmacological and genetic studies in the mouse.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Boyland NK, Mlynski DT, James R, Brent LJN, Croft DP (2016) The social network structure of a dynamic group of dairy cows: from individual to group level patterns. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 174, 1–10.
The social network structure of a dynamic group of dairy cows: from individual to group level patterns.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Brambell FWR (1965) Report of the technical committee to enquire into the welfare of animals kept under intensive husbandry systems. Cmnd 2836. HM Stationery Office, London, UK.

Brandle JR, Quam V, Johnson L, Wight B (1994) ‘EC94-1766 windbreaks for livestock operations.’ Historical materials from University of Nebraska–Lincoln Extension: 840. Available at http://digitalcommons.unl.edu/extensionhist/840 [Accessed 11 April 2018]

Brower GR, Kiracofe GH (1978) Factors associated with the Buller-steer syndrome. Journal of Animal Science 46, 26–31.
Factors associated with the Buller-steer syndrome.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Brown-Brandl TM, Eigenberg RA, Nienaber JA, Hahn GL (2005) Dynamic response indicators of heat stress in shaded and non-shaded feedlot cattle, part 1: analyses of indicators. Biosystems Engineering 90, 451–462.
Dynamic response indicators of heat stress in shaded and non-shaded feedlot cattle, part 1: analyses of indicators.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Brown-Brandl TM, Eigenberg RA, Nienaber JA (2006a) Heat stress risk factors of feedlot heifers. Livestock Science 105, 57–68.
Heat stress risk factors of feedlot heifers.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Brown-Brandl TM, Nienaber JA, Eigenberg RA, Mader TL, Morrow JL, Dailey JW (2006b) Comparison of heat tolerance of feedlot heifers of different breeds. Livestock Science 105, 19–26.
Comparison of heat tolerance of feedlot heifers of different breeds.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Brown-Brandl TM, Eigenberg RA, Nienaber JA (2010) Water spray cooling during handling of feedlot cattle. International Journal of Biometeorology 54, 609–616.
Water spray cooling during handling of feedlot cattle.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 19960206PubMed |

Buhman MJ, Hungerford LL, Smith DR (2003) An economic risk assessment of the management of pregnant feedlot heifers in the USA. Preventive Veterinary Medicine 59, 207–222.
An economic risk assessment of the management of pregnant feedlot heifers in the USA.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 12835005PubMed |

Burrow HM (1997) Measurements of temperament and their relationships with performance traits of beef cattle. Animal Breeding Abstracts 65, 477–495.

Burrow HM, Dillon RD (1997) Relationships between temperament and growth in a feedlot and commercial carcass traits of Bos indicus crossbreds. Australian Journal of Experimental Agriculture 37, 407–411.
Relationships between temperament and growth in a feedlot and commercial carcass traits of Bos indicus crossbreds.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Burrow HM, Seifert GW, Corbet NJ (1988) A new technique for measuring temperament in cattle. Proceedings of the Australian Society of Animal Production 17, 154–157.

Bushby EV, Friel M, Goold C, Gray H, Smith L, Collins LM (2018) Factors influencing individual variation in farm animal cognition and how to account for these statistically. Frontiers in Veterinary Science 5, 193
Factors influencing individual variation in farm animal cognition and how to account for these statistically.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 30175105PubMed |

Cafe LM, Robinson DL, Ferguson DM, Geesink GH, Greenwood PL (2011a) Temperament and hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis function are related and combine to affect growth, efficiency, carcass, and meat quality traits in Brahman steers. Domestic Animal Endocrinology 40, 230–240.
Temperament and hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis function are related and combine to affect growth, efficiency, carcass, and meat quality traits in Brahman steers.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 21414739PubMed |

Cafe LM, Robinson DL, Ferguson DM, McIntyre BL, Geesink GH, Greenwood PL (2011b) Cattle temperament: persistence of assessments and associations with productivity, efficiency, carcass and meat quality traits. Journal of Animal Science 89, 1452–1465.
Cattle temperament: persistence of assessments and associations with productivity, efficiency, carcass and meat quality traits.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 21169516PubMed |

Castaneda CA, Gaughan JB, Sakaguchi Y (2004) Relationships between climatic conditions and the behavior of feedlot cattle. Animal Production in Australia 25, 33–36.

Ceballos MC, Sant’Anna AC, Boivin X, Costa FdO, Carvalhal MV, de Paranhos L, da Costa MJR (2018) Impact of good practices of handling training on beef cattle welfare and stockpeople attitudes and behaviors. Livestock Science 216, 24–31.
Impact of good practices of handling training on beef cattle welfare and stockpeople attitudes and behaviors.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Charlton GL, Rutter SM, East M, Sinclair LA (2013) The motivation of dairy cows for access to pasture. Journal of Dairy Science 96, 4387–4396.
The motivation of dairy cows for access to pasture.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 23664348PubMed |

Chen JM, Schütz KE, Tucker CB (2013) Dairy cows use and prefer feed bunks fitted with sprinklers. Journal of Dairy Science 96, 5035–5045.
Dairy cows use and prefer feed bunks fitted with sprinklers.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 23769371PubMed |

Chen JM, Schütz KE, Tucker CB (2016) Cooling cows efficiently with water spray: behavioral, physiological, and production responses to sprinklers at the feed bunk. Journal of Dairy Science 99, 4607–4618.
Cooling cows efficiently with water spray: behavioral, physiological, and production responses to sprinklers at the feed bunk.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 27060833PubMed |

Church JS, Hegadoren P, Paetkau M, Miller C, Regev-Shoshani G, Schaefer A, Schwartzkopf-Genswein K (2014) Influence of environmental factors on infrared eye temperature measurements in cattle. Research in Veterinary Science 96, 220–226.
Influence of environmental factors on infrared eye temperature measurements in cattle.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 24290729PubMed |

Clarke M, Kelly A (1996) Some effects of shade on Hereford steers in a feedlot. In ‘Proceedings – Australian Society of Animal Production 21st biennial conference’, 8–12 July 1996, The University of Queensland, Qld, Australia. pp. 235–238. (Australian Society of Animal Production: Armidale, NSW)

Colditz IG (2004) Some mechanisms regulating nutrient utilisation in livestock during immune activation: an overview. Australian Journal of Experimental Agriculture 44, 453–458.
Some mechanisms regulating nutrient utilisation in livestock during immune activation: an overview.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Colditz IG, Hine BC (2016) Resilience in farm animals: biology, management, breeding and implications for animal welfare. Animal Production Science 56, 1961–1983.
Resilience in farm animals: biology, management, breeding and implications for animal welfare.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Colditz IG, Ferguson DM, Greenwood PL, Doogan VJ, Petherick JC, Kilgour RJ (2007) Regrouping unfamiliar animals in the weeks prior to slaughter has few effects on physiology and meat quality in Bos taurus feedlot steers. Australian Journal of Experimental Agriculture 47, 763–769.
Regrouping unfamiliar animals in the weeks prior to slaughter has few effects on physiology and meat quality in Bos taurus feedlot steers.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Colditz I, Ferguson D, Collins T, Matthews L, Hemsworth P (2014) A prototype tool to enable farmers to measure and improve the welfare performance of the farm animal enterprise: the unified field index. Animals 4, 446–462.
A prototype tool to enable farmers to measure and improve the welfare performance of the farm animal enterprise: the unified field index.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 26480317PubMed |

Coleman G, Hemsworth P, Hay M, Cox M (2000) Modifying stockperson attitudes and behaviour towards pigs at a large commercial farm. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 66, 11–20.
Modifying stockperson attitudes and behaviour towards pigs at a large commercial farm.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Comin A, Peric T, Corazzin M, Veronesi MC, Meloni T, Zufferli V, Cornacchia G, Prandi A (2013) Hair cortisol as a marker of hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis activation in Friesian dairy cows clinically or physiologically compromised. Livestock Science 152, 36–41.
Hair cortisol as a marker of hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis activation in Friesian dairy cows clinically or physiologically compromised.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Creutzinger KC, Stookey JM, Marfleet TW, Campbell JR, Janz DM, Marques FJ, Seddon YM (2017) An investigation of hair cortisol as a measure of long-term stress in beef cattle: results from a castration study. Canadian Journal of Animal Science 97, 499–509.
An investigation of hair cortisol as a measure of long-term stress in beef cattle: results from a castration study.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Crump A, Arnott G, Bethell E (2018) Affect-driven attention biases as animal welfare indicators: review and methods. Animals 8, 136
Affect-driven attention biases as animal welfare indicators: review and methods.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Cusack PMV, McMeniman NP, Lean IJ (2007) Feedlot entry characteristics and climate: their relationship with cattle growth rate, bovine respiratory disease and mortality. Australian Veterinary Journal 85, 311–316.
Feedlot entry characteristics and climate: their relationship with cattle growth rate, bovine respiratory disease and mortality.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Daigle CL, Ridge EE (2018) Investing in stockpeople is an investment in animal welfare and agricultural sustainability. Animal Frontiers 8, 53–59.
Investing in stockpeople is an investment in animal welfare and agricultural sustainability.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Daigle CL, Jackson B, Gill R, Wickersham TA, Sawyer JE (2017) Impact of exercise on productivity, behavior, and immune functioning of weaned Bos indicus-cross calves housed in drylots. Journal of Animal Science 95, 5230–5239.
Impact of exercise on productivity, behavior, and immune functioning of weaned Bos indicus-cross calves housed in drylots.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 29293791PubMed |

Daros RR, Costa JHC, von Keyserlingk MAG, Hotzel MJ, Weary DM (2014) Separation from the dam causes negative judgement bias in dairy calves. PLoS One 9, e98429
Separation from the dam causes negative judgement bias in dairy calves.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 24848635PubMed |

Davis MS, Mader TL, Holt SM, Parkhurst AM (2003) Strategies to reduce feedlot cattle heat stress: effects on tympanic temperature. Journal of Animal Science 81, 649–661.
Strategies to reduce feedlot cattle heat stress: effects on tympanic temperature.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 12661645PubMed |

Davis-Unger J, Schwartzkopf-Genswein K, Pajor EA, Hendrick S, Marti S, Dorin C, Orsel K (2019) Prevalence and lameness-associated risk factors in Alberta feedlot cattle Translational Animal Science 3, 595–606.
Prevalence and lameness-associated risk factors in Alberta feedlot cattleCrossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

DeVries TJ, Schwaiger T, Beauchemin KA, Penner GB (2014) The duration of time that beef cattle are fed a high-grain diet affects feed sorting behavior, both prior to and after acute ruminal acidosis. Journal of Animal Science 92, 1728–1737.
The duration of time that beef cattle are fed a high-grain diet affects feed sorting behavior, both prior to and after acute ruminal acidosis.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 24663165PubMed |

Dikmen S, Ustuner H, Orman A (2012) The effect of body weight on some welfare indicators in feedlot cattle in a hot environment. International Journal of Biometeorology 56, 297–303.
The effect of body weight on some welfare indicators in feedlot cattle in a hot environment.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 21533672PubMed |

Douglas C, Bateson M, Walsh C, Bedue A, Edwards SA (2012) Environmental enrichment induces optimistic cognitive biases in pigs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 139, 65–73.
Environmental enrichment induces optimistic cognitive biases in pigs.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Doyle RE, Fisher AD, Hinch GN, Boissy A, Lee C (2010) Release from restraint generates a positive judgement bias in sheep. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 122, 28–34.
Release from restraint generates a positive judgement bias in sheep.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Edwards TA (1995) Buller syndrome: what’s behind this abnormal sexual behavior? Large Animal Veterinarian 50, 6–8.

Edwards TA (2010) Control methods for bovine respiratory disease for feedlot cattle. Veterinary Clinics: Food Animal Practice 26, 273–284.
Control methods for bovine respiratory disease for feedlot cattle.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Eigenberg RA, Brown-Brandl TM, Nienaber JA, Hahn GL (2005) Dynamic response indicators of heat stress in shaded and non-shaded feedlot cattle, Part 2: predictive relationships. Biosystems Engineering 91, 111–118.
Dynamic response indicators of heat stress in shaded and non-shaded feedlot cattle, Part 2: predictive relationships.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Eigenberg RA, Brown-Brandl TM, Nienaber JA (2010) Shade material evaluation using a cattle response model and meteorological instrumentation. International Journal of Biometeorology 54, 601–607.
Shade material evaluation using a cattle response model and meteorological instrumentation.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 21061024PubMed |

Elgersma GG, De Jong G, Van der Linde R, Mulder HA (2018) Fluctuations in milk yield are heritable and can be used as a resilience indicator to breed healthy cows. Journal of Dairy Science 101, 1240–1250.
Fluctuations in milk yield are heritable and can be used as a resilience indicator to breed healthy cows.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 29174159PubMed |

Entwistle K, Rose M, McKiernan B (2000) ‘Mortalities in feedlot cattle at prime city feedlot, Tabbita, NSW, February 2000.’ (NSW Agriculture: Sydney, NSW, Australia)

Eurobarometer (2007) ‘Attitudes of EU citizens towards animal welfare.’ European commission. Available at http://ec.europa.eu/commfrontoffice/publicopinion/archives/ebs/ebs_270_en.pdf. [Verified 5 November 2018]

Farm Animal Welfare Council (2009) ‘Farm animal welfare in Great Britain: past, present and future.’ (Farm Animal Welfare Council, London, UK)

Fell LR, Colditz IG, Walker KH, Watson DL (1999) Associations between temperament, performance and immune function in cattle entering a commercial feedlot. Australian Journal of Experimental Agriculture 39, 795–802.
Associations between temperament, performance and immune function in cattle entering a commercial feedlot.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Ferguson DM, Johnston D, Burrow HM, Reverter A (2006) Relationships between temperament, feedlot performance and beef quality. In ‘Australian beef: the leader’, (Ed. H Burrow) pp. 161–165) (CRC for Beef Genetic Technologies: Armidale, NSW, Australia)

Finkemeier M-A, Langbein J, Puppe B (2018) Personality research in mammalian farm animals: concepts, measures, and relationship to welfare. Frontiers in Veterinary Science 5, 131
Personality research in mammalian farm animals: concepts, measures, and relationship to welfare.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 30003083PubMed |

Fisher AD, Verkerk GA, Morrow CJ, Matthews LR (2002) The effects of feed restriction and lying deprivation on pituitary–adrenal axis regulation in lactating cows. Livestock Production Science 73, 255–263.
The effects of feed restriction and lying deprivation on pituitary–adrenal axis regulation in lactating cows.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Fleming PA, Wickham SL, Stockman CA, Verbeek E, Matthews L, Wemelsfelder F (2015) The sensitivity of QBA assessments of sheep behavioral expression to variations in visual or verbal information provided to observers. Animal 9, 878–887.
The sensitivity of QBA assessments of sheep behavioral expression to variations in visual or verbal information provided to observers.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 25583376PubMed |

Fleming PA, Clarke T, Wickham SL, Stockman CA, Barnes AL, Collins T, Miller DW (2016) The contribution of qualitative behavioral assessment to appraisal of livestock welfare. Animal Production Science 56, 1569–1578.
The contribution of qualitative behavioral assessment to appraisal of livestock welfare.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Fordyce G, Goddard M, Tyler R, Williams G, Toleman M (1985) Temperament and bruising of Bos indicus cross cattle. Australian Journal of Experimental Agriculture 25, 283–288.
Temperament and bruising of Bos indicus cross cattle.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Fordyce G, Wythes J, Shorthose W, Underwood D, Shepherd R (1988) Cattle temperaments in extensive beef herds in northern Queensland. 2. Effect of temperament on carcass and meat quality. Australian Journal of Experimental Agriculture 28, 689–693.
Cattle temperaments in extensive beef herds in northern Queensland. 2. Effect of temperament on carcass and meat quality.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Forkman B, Boissy A, Meunier-Salauen MC, Canali E, Jones RB (2007) A critical review of fear tests used on cattle, pigs, sheep, poultry and horses. Physiology and Behavior 92, 340–374.
A critical review of fear tests used on cattle, pigs, sheep, poultry and horses.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 18046784PubMed |

Fox JT (2015) Management of feedyard hospitals. Veterinary Clinics: Food Animal Practice 31, 455–463.

Fraser D (2008) Toward a global perspective on farm animal welfare. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 113, 330–339.
Toward a global perspective on farm animal welfare.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Fureix C, Meagher RK (2015) What can inactivity (in its various forms) reveal about affective states in non-human animals? A review. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 171, 8–24.
What can inactivity (in its various forms) reveal about affective states in non-human animals? A review.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Galyean ML, Rivera JD (2003) Nutritionally related disorders affecting feedlot cattle. Canadian Journal of Animal Science 83, 13–20.
Nutritionally related disorders affecting feedlot cattle.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Gardner BA, Dolezal HG, Bryant LK, Owens FN, Smith RA (1999) Health of finishing steers: effects on performance, carcass traits, and meat tenderness. Journal of Animal Science 77, 3168–3175.
Health of finishing steers: effects on performance, carcass traits, and meat tenderness.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 10641860PubMed |

Gaughan JB, Mader TL (2014) Body temperature and respiratory dynamics in un-shaded beef cattle. International Journal of Biometeorology 58, 1443–1450.
Body temperature and respiratory dynamics in un-shaded beef cattle.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 24122341PubMed |

Gaughan JB, Sullivan ML (2014) Australian feedlot industry. In ‘Beef cattle production and trade’. (Eds L Kahn, and D Cottle) pp. 205–233. (CSIRO Publishing: Melbourne, Vic., Australia)

Gaughan JB, Davis MS, Mader TL (2004a) Wetting and the physiological responses of grain-fed cattle in a heated environment. Australian Journal of Agricultural Research 55, 253–260.
Wetting and the physiological responses of grain-fed cattle in a heated environment.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Gaughan JB, Tait LA, Eigenberg R, Bryden WL (2004b) Effect of shade on respiration rate and rectal temperature of Angus heifers. Animal Production in Australia 1, 69–72.

Gaughan JB, Mader TL, Holt SM (2008a) Cooling and feeding strategies to reduce heat load of grain-fed beef cattle in intensive housing. Livestock Science 113, 226–233.
Cooling and feeding strategies to reduce heat load of grain-fed beef cattle in intensive housing.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Gaughan JB, Mader TL, Holt SM, Lisle A (2008b) A new heat load index for feedlot cattle. Journal of Animal Science 86, 226–234.
A new heat load index for feedlot cattle.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 17911236PubMed |

Gaughan JB, Bonner S, Loxton I, Mader TL, Lisle A, Lawrence R (2010) Effect of shade on body temperature and performance of feedlot steers. Journal of Animal Science 88, 4056–4067.
Effect of shade on body temperature and performance of feedlot steers.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 20709874PubMed |

Gaughan JB, Bonner S, Loxton I, Mader TL (2013) Effects of chronic heat stress on plasma concentration of secreted heat shock protein 70 in growing feedlot cattle. Journal of Animal Science 91, 120–129.
Effects of chronic heat stress on plasma concentration of secreted heat shock protein 70 in growing feedlot cattle.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 23048154PubMed |

Góis KCR, Ceballos MC, Sant’Anna AC, Paranhos de Costa MJR (2016) Using an observer rating method to assess the effects of rotational stocking method on beef cattle temperament over time. Revista Brasileira de Zootecnia 45, 501–508.
Using an observer rating method to assess the effects of rotational stocking method on beef cattle temperament over time.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Gómez Y, Bieler R, Hankele AK, Zähner M, Savary P, Hillmann E (2018) Evaluation of visible eye white and maximum eye temperature as non-invasive indicators of stress in dairy cows. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 198, 1–8.
Evaluation of visible eye white and maximum eye temperature as non-invasive indicators of stress in dairy cows.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

González LA, Ferret A, Manteca X, Ruíz-de-la-Torre JL, Calsamiglia S, Devant M, Bach A (2008) Effect of the number of concentrate feeding places per pen on performance, behavior, and welfare indicators of Friesian calves during the first month after arrival at the feedlot. Journal of Animal Science 86, 419–431.
Effect of the number of concentrate feeding places per pen on performance, behavior, and welfare indicators of Friesian calves during the first month after arrival at the feedlot.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 17940151PubMed |

Grandin T (1993) Behavioral agitation during handling of cattle is persistent over time. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 36, 1–9.
Behavioral agitation during handling of cattle is persistent over time.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Grandin T (1997) The design and construction of facilities for handling cattle. Livestock Production Science 49, 103–119.
The design and construction of facilities for handling cattle.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Grandin T (2016) Evaluation of the welfare of cattle housed in outdoor feedlot pens. Veterinary and Animal Science 1–2, 23–28.
Evaluation of the welfare of cattle housed in outdoor feedlot pens.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Gutmann A, Schwed B, Tremetsberger L, Winckler C (2015a) Intra-day variation of qualitative behavior assessment outcomes in dairy cattle. Animal Welfare 24, 319–326.
Intra-day variation of qualitative behavior assessment outcomes in dairy cattle.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Gutmann AK, Špinka M, Winckler C (2015b) Long-term familiarity creates preferred social partners in dairy cows. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 169, 1–8.
Long-term familiarity creates preferred social partners in dairy cows.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Hahn GL (1999) Dynamic responses of cattle to thermal heat loads. Journal of Animal Science 77, 10–20.
Dynamic responses of cattle to thermal heat loads.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 15526777PubMed |

Hahn GL, Mader TL (1997) Heat waves in relation to thermoregulation, feeding behavior and mortality of feedlot cattle. In ‘Proceedings of the fifth international livestock environment symposium’. (Eds RW Bottcher, SJ Hoff) pp. 563–571. (American Society of Agricultural Engineers, St Joseph, MI, USA)

Hansson H, Lagerkvist CJ (2014) Defining and measuring farmers’ attitudes to farm animal welfare. Animal Welfare 23, 47–56.
Defining and measuring farmers’ attitudes to farm animal welfare.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Hay KE, Barnes TS, Morton JM, Gravel JL, Commins MA, Horwood PF, Ambrose RC, Clements ACA, Mahony TJ (2016) Associations between exposure to viruses and bovine respiratory disease in Australian feedlot cattle. Preventive Veterinary Medicine 127, 121–133.
Associations between exposure to viruses and bovine respiratory disease in Australian feedlot cattle.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 26972273PubMed |

Held SDE, Špinka M (2011) Animal play and animal welfare. Animal Behaviour 81, 891–899.
Animal play and animal welfare.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Hemsworth PH (2003) Human–animal interactions in livestock production. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 81, 185–198.
Human–animal interactions in livestock production.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Hemsworth PH, Barnett JL, Coleman GJ (1993) The human–animal relationship in agriculture and its consequences for the animal. Animal Welfare 2, 33–51.

Hemsworth PH, Mellor DJ, Cronin GM, Tilbrook AJ (2015) Scientific assessment of animal welfare. New Zealand Veterinary Journal 63, 24–30.
Scientific assessment of animal welfare.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 25263963PubMed |

Henry B, Charmley E, Eckard R, Gaughan JB, Hegarty R (2012) Livestock production in a changing climate: adaptation and mitigation research in Australia. Crop and Pasture Science 63, 191–202.
Livestock production in a changing climate: adaptation and mitigation research in Australia.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Herring AD (2014) ‘Beef cattle production systems.’ (CABI: Boston, MA, USA)

Hine BC, Bell AM, Niemeyer DDO, Duff CJ, Butcher NM, Dominik S, Ingham AB, Colditz IG (2019) Immune competence traits assessed during the stress of weaning are heritable and favourably genetically correlated with temperament traits in Angus cattle. Journal of Animal Science 97, 4053–4065.
Immune competence traits assessed during the stress of weaning are heritable and favourably genetically correlated with temperament traits in Angus cattle.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 31581299PubMed |

Irwin MR, Melendy DR, Amoss MS, Hutcheson DP (1979) Roles of predisposing factors and gonadal hormones in the Buller syndrome of feedlot steers. Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Association 174, 367–370.

Ishiwata T, Uetake K, Abe N, Eguchi Y, Tanaka T (2006) Effects of an environmental enrichment using a drum can on behavioral, physiological and productive characteristics in fattening beef cattle. Animal Science Journal 77, 352–362.
Effects of an environmental enrichment using a drum can on behavioral, physiological and productive characteristics in fattening beef cattle.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Ishiwata T, Uetake K, Eguchi Y, Tanaka T (2008) Function of tongue-playing of cattle in association with other behavioral and physiological characteristics. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 11, 358–367.
Function of tongue-playing of cattle in association with other behavioral and physiological characteristics.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 18821405PubMed |

Jensen MB (2018) The role of social behavior in cattle welfare. In ‘Advances in cattle welfare’. (Ed. CB Tucker) pp. 123–155. (Woodhead Publishing, Duxford, UK)

Kadel MJ, Johnston DJ, Burrow HM, Graser H-U, Ferguson DM (2006) Genetics of flight time and other measures of temperament and their value as selection criteria for improving meat quality traits in tropically adapted breeds of beef cattle. Australian Journal of Agricultural Research 57, 1029–1035.
Genetics of flight time and other measures of temperament and their value as selection criteria for improving meat quality traits in tropically adapted breeds of beef cattle.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Kappel S, Mendl MT, Barrett DC, Murrell JC, Whay HR (2017) Lateralized behavior as indicator of affective state in dairy cows. PLoS One 12, e0184933
Lateralized behavior as indicator of affective state in dairy cows.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 28910416PubMed |

Kilgour RJ (2012) In pursuit of ‘normal’: a review of the behavior of cattle at pasture. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 138, 1–11.
In pursuit of ‘normal’: a review of the behavior of cattle at pasture.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Kilgour RJ, Melville GJ, Greenwood PL (2006) Individual differences in the reaction of beef cattle to situations involving social isolation, close proximity of humans, restraint and novelty. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 99, 21–40.
Individual differences in the reaction of beef cattle to situations involving social isolation, close proximity of humans, restraint and novelty.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

King DA, Schuehle Pfeiffer CE, Randel RD, Welsh TH, Oliphint RA, Baird BE, Curley KO, Vann RC, Hale DS, Savell JW (2006) Influence of animal temperament and stress responsiveness on the carcass quality and beef tenderness of feedlot cattle. Meat Science 74, 546–556.
Influence of animal temperament and stress responsiveness on the carcass quality and beef tenderness of feedlot cattle.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 22063059PubMed |

Kirkden RD, Pajor EA (2006) Using preference, motivation and aversion tests to ask scientific questions about animals’ feelings. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 100, 29–47.
Using preference, motivation and aversion tests to ask scientific questions about animals’ feelings.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Knierim U, Winckler C (2009) On-farm welfare assessment in cattle: validity, reliability and feasibility issues and future perspectives with special regard to the Welfare Quality® approach. Animal Welfare 18, 451–458.

Koolhaas JM, Korte SM, De Boer SF, Van Der Vegt BJ, Van Reenen CG, Hopster H, De Jong IC, Ruis MAW, Blokhuis HJ (1999) Coping styles in animals: current status in behavior and stress-physiology. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 23, 925–935.
Coping styles in animals: current status in behavior and stress-physiology.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 10580307PubMed |

Lambert HS, Carder G (2017) Looking into the eyes of a cow: can eye whites be used as a measure of emotional state? Applied Animal Behaviour Science 186, 1–6.
Looking into the eyes of a cow: can eye whites be used as a measure of emotional state?Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Lee C, Fisher AD, Colditz IG, Lea JM, Ferguson DM (2013) Preference of beef cattle for feedlot or pasture environments. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 145, 53–59.
Preference of beef cattle for feedlot or pasture environments.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Lee C, Lea J, Thomson P, Pool K, Colditz I (2016a) ‘The effect of mud on cattle motivation for feedlot or pasture environments.’ (Meat and Livestock Australia: Sydney, NSW, Australia)

Lee C, Verbeek E, Doyle R, Bateson M (2016b) Attention bias to threat indicates anxiety differences in sheep. Biology Letters 12, 20150977
Attention bias to threat indicates anxiety differences in sheep.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 27277950PubMed |

Lee C, Cafe LM, Robinson SL, Doyle RE, Lea JM, Small AH, Colditz IG (2018) Anxiety influences attention bias but not flight speed and crush score in beef cattle. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 205, 210–215.
Anxiety influences attention bias but not flight speed and crush score in beef cattle.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Lees A, Lea J, Salvin H, Cafe L, Colditz I, Lee C (2018a) Relationship between rectal temperature and vaginal temperature in grazing Bos taurus heifers. Animals 8, 156
Relationship between rectal temperature and vaginal temperature in grazing Bos taurus heifers.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Lees AM, Lees JC, Lisle AT, Sullivan ML, Gaughan JB (2018b) Effect of heat stress on rumen temperature of three breeds of cattle. International Journal of Biometeorology 62, 207–215.
Effect of heat stress on rumen temperature of three breeds of cattle.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 28918576PubMed |

Lees AM, Lees JC, Sejian V, Sullivan ML, Gaughan JB (2020) Influence of shade on panting score and behavioural responses of Bos taurus and Bos indicus feedlot cattle to heat load. Animal Production Science 60, 305–315.
Influence of shade on panting score and behavioural responses of Bos taurus and Bos indicus feedlot cattle to heat load.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Lefcourt AM, Adams WR (1996) Radiotelemetry measurement of body temperatures of feedlot steers during summer. Journal of Animal Science 74, 2633–2640.
Radiotelemetry measurement of body temperatures of feedlot steers during summer.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 8923177PubMed |

Lefcourt AM, Huntington JB, Akers RM, Wood DL, Bitman J (1999) Circadian and ultradian rhythms of body temperature and peripheral concentrations of insulin and nitrogen in lactating dairy cows. Domestic Animal Endocrinology 16, 41–55.
Circadian and ultradian rhythms of body temperature and peripheral concentrations of insulin and nitrogen in lactating dairy cows.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 10081662PubMed |

Legrand A, Schütz KE, Tucker CB (2011) Using water to cool cattle: behavioral and physiological changes associated with voluntary use of cow showers. Journal of Dairy Science 94, 3376–3386.
Using water to cool cattle: behavioral and physiological changes associated with voluntary use of cow showers.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 21700023PubMed |

Leliveld LMC, Langbein J, Puppe B (2013) The emergence of emotional lateralization: evidence in non-human vertebrates and implications for farm animals. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 145, 1–14.
The emergence of emotional lateralization: evidence in non-human vertebrates and implications for farm animals.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Li Y, Wang L (2011) Effects of previous housing system on agonistic behaviors of growing pigs at mixing. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 132, 20–26.
Effects of previous housing system on agonistic behaviors of growing pigs at mixing.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Lyles JL, Calvo-Lorenzo MS (2014) Bill E. Kunkle interdisciplinary beef symposium: practical developments in managing animal welfare in beef cattle: what does the future hold? Journal of Animal Science 92, 5334–5344.
Bill E. Kunkle interdisciplinary beef symposium: practical developments in managing animal welfare in beef cattle: what does the future hold?Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 25253809PubMed |

MacKay J, Haskell M (2015) Consistent individual behavioral variation: the difference between temperament, personality and behavioral syndromes. Animals 5, 366
Consistent individual behavioral variation: the difference between temperament, personality and behavioral syndromes.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

MacKay JRD, Turner SP, Hyslop J, Deag JM, Haskell MJ (2013) Short-term temperament tests in beef cattle relate to long-term measures of behavior recorded in the home pen. Journal of Animal Science 91, 4917–4924.
Short-term temperament tests in beef cattle relate to long-term measures of behavior recorded in the home pen.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Mader TL (2003) Environmental stress in confined beef cattle. Journal of Animal Science 81, E110–E119.

Mader TL (2011) Mud effects on feedlot cattle. Nebraska beef cattle reports. 613. Available at https://digitalcommons.unl.edu/animalscinbcr/613/. [Verified 23 April 2018]

Mader TL, Colgan SL (2007) Pen density and straw bedding during feedlot finishing. Nebraska beef cattle reports. 70. Available at https://digitalcommons.unl.edu/animalscinbcr/70. [Verified 28 March 2019]

Mader TL, Davis MS (2004) Effect of management strategies on reducing heat stress of feedlot cattle: feed and water intake. Journal of Animal Science 82, 3077–3087.
Effect of management strategies on reducing heat stress of feedlot cattle: feed and water intake.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 15484961PubMed |

Mader TL, Griffin D (2015) Management of cattle exposed to adverse environmental conditions. The Veterinary Clinics of North America. Food Animal Practice 31, 247–258.
Management of cattle exposed to adverse environmental conditions.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 26139190PubMed |

Mader TL, Holt SM, Hahn GL, Davis MS, Spiers DE (2002) Feeding strategies for managing heat load in feedlot cattle. Journal of Animal Science 80, 2373–2382.
Feeding strategies for managing heat load in feedlot cattle.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 12350014PubMed |

Mader TL, Davis MS, Brown-Brandl TM (2006) Environmental factors influencing heat stress in feedlot cattle. Journal of Animal Science 84, 712–719.
Environmental factors influencing heat stress in feedlot cattle.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 16478964PubMed |

Mader TL, Davis MS, Gaughan JB (2007) Effect of sprinkling on feedlot microclimate and cattle behavior. International Journal of Biometeorology 51, 541–551.
Effect of sprinkling on feedlot microclimate and cattle behavior.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 17364184PubMed |

Mader TL, Gaughan JB, Johnson LJ, Hahn GL (2010) Tympanic temperature in confined beef cattle exposed to excessive heat load. International Journal of Biometeorology 54, 629–635.
Tympanic temperature in confined beef cattle exposed to excessive heat load.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 19404683PubMed |

Maia A, da Silva RG, Loureiro CB (2005) Sensible and latent heat loss from the body surface of Holstein cows in a tropical environment. International Journal of Biometeorology 50, 17–22.
Sensible and latent heat loss from the body surface of Holstein cows in a tropical environment.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 15864465PubMed |

Mason G, Mendl M (1993) Why is there no simple way of measuring animal welfare? Animal Welfare 2, 301–319.

Mazurek M, McGee M, Minchin W, Crowe MA, Earley B (2011) Is the avoidance distance test for the assessment of animals’ responsiveness to humans influenced by either the dominant or flightiest animal in the group? Applied Animal Behaviour Science 132, 107–113.
Is the avoidance distance test for the assessment of animals’ responsiveness to humans influenced by either the dominant or flightiest animal in the group?Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

McDowell RE, Weldy JR (1967) Water exchange of cattle under heat stress. In ‘Proceedings of the 3rd international biometeorological congress’ London. pp. 414–424. (Symposium Publications Division, Pergamon Press: Oxford, NY).

McPhee CP, McBride G, James JW (1964) Social behavior of domestic animals III. Steers in small yards. Animal Science 6, 9–15.
Social behavior of domestic animals III. Steers in small yards.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Meagher RK, von Keyserlingk MAG, Atkinson D, Weary DM (2016) Inconsistency in dairy calves’ responses to tests of fearfulness. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 185, 15–22.
Inconsistency in dairy calves’ responses to tests of fearfulness.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Mellor DJ (2015) Positive animal welfare states and encouraging environment-focused and animal-to-animal interactive behaviors. New Zealand Veterinary Journal 63, 9–16.
Positive animal welfare states and encouraging environment-focused and animal-to-animal interactive behaviors.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 24875367PubMed |

Mellor D, Beausoleil N (2015) Extending the ‘five domains’ model for animal welfare assessment to incorporate positive welfare states. Animal Welfare 24, 241–253.
Extending the ‘five domains’ model for animal welfare assessment to incorporate positive welfare states.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Mench JA, Swanson JC, Stricklin WR (1990) Social stress and dominance among group members after mixing beef cows. Canadian Journal of Animal Science 70, 345–354.
Social stress and dominance among group members after mixing beef cows.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Mendl M, Burman OHP, Paul ES (2010) An integrative and functional framework for the study of animal emotion and mood. Proceedings. Biological Sciences 277, 2895
An integrative and functional framework for the study of animal emotion and mood.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 20685706PubMed |

Meyer JS, Novak MA (2012) Minireview: hair cortisol: a novel biomarker of hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenocortical activity. Endocrinology 153, 4120–4127.
Minireview: hair cortisol: a novel biomarker of hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenocortical activity.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 22778226PubMed |

Mitlöhner FM, Morrow JL, Dailey JW, Wilson SC, Galyean ML, Miller MF, McGlone JJ (2001) Shade and water misting effects on behavior, physiology, performance, and carcass traits of heat-stressed feedlot cattle. Journal of Animal Science 79, 2327–2335.
Shade and water misting effects on behavior, physiology, performance, and carcass traits of heat-stressed feedlot cattle.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 11583419PubMed |

Mitlöhner FM, Galyean ML, McGlone JJ (2002) Shade effects on performance, carcass traits, physiology, and behavior of heat-stressed feedlot heifers. Journal of Animal Science 80, 2043–2050.
Shade effects on performance, carcass traits, physiology, and behavior of heat-stressed feedlot heifers.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 12211371PubMed |

Moberg GP (2000) Biological response to stress: implications for animal welfare. In ‘The biology of animal stress’. (Eds GP Moberg, JA Mench) pp. 1–21. (CABI Publishing: Wallingford, UK)

Monk JE, Hine BC, Colditz IG, Lee C (2018a) A novel protocol to assess acclimation rate in Bos taurus heifers during yard weaning. Animals 8, 51
A novel protocol to assess acclimation rate in Bos taurus heifers during yard weaning.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Monk JE, Doyle RE, Colditz IG, Belson S, Cronin GM, Lee C (2018b) Towards a more practical attention bias test to assess affective state in sheep. PLoS One 13, e0190404
Towards a more practical attention bias test to assess affective state in sheep.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 29293636PubMed |

Monk JE, Belson S, Colditz IG, Lee C (2018c) Attention bias test differentiates anxiety and depression in sheep. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience 12, 246
Attention bias test differentiates anxiety and depression in sheep.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 30405371PubMed |

Montillo M, Comin A, Corazzin M, Peric T, Faustini M, Veronesi MC, Valentini S, Bustaffa M, Prandi A (2014) The effect of temperature, rainfall, and light conditions on hair cortisol concentrations in newborn foals. Journal of Equine Veterinary Science 34, 774–778.
The effect of temperature, rainfall, and light conditions on hair cortisol concentrations in newborn foals.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Morrison SR, Givens RL, Lofgreen GP (1973) Sprinkling cattle for relief from heat stress. Journal of Animal Science 36, 428–431.
Sprinkling cattle for relief from heat stress.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Mounier L, Veissier I, Andanson S, Delval E, Boissy A (2006) Mixing at the beginning of fattening moderates social buffering in beef bulls. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 96, 185–200.
Mixing at the beginning of fattening moderates social buffering in beef bulls.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Moya D, Schwartzkopf-Genswein KS, Veira DM (2013) Standardization of a non-invasive methodology to measure cortisol in hair of beef cattle. Livestock Science 158, 138–144.
Standardization of a non-invasive methodology to measure cortisol in hair of beef cattle.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Müller R, von Keyserlingk MAG (2006) Consistency of flight speed and its correlation to productivity and to personality in Bos taurus beef cattle. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 99, 193–204.
Consistency of flight speed and its correlation to productivity and to personality in Bos taurus beef cattle.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Munksgaard L, Jensen MB, Pedersen LJ, Hansen SW, Matthews L (2005) Quantifying behavioural priorities – effects of time constraints on behaviour of dairy cows, Bos taurus. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 92, 3–14.
Quantifying behavioural priorities – effects of time constraints on behaviour of dairy cows, Bos taurus.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Nardone A, Ronchi B, Lacetera N, Ranieri MS, Bernabucci U (2010) Effects of climate changes on animal production and sustainability of livestock systems. Livestock Science 130, 57–69.
Effects of climate changes on animal production and sustainability of livestock systems.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

National Guidelines for Beef Cattle Feedlots in Australia (2012) ‘National guidelines for beef feedlots in Australia. ‘(Meat and Livestock Australia: Sydney, NSW, Australia)

National Research Council (2000) ‘Nutrient requirements of beef cattle.’ (National Research Council: Washington DC, USA)

Neave HW, Daros RR, Costa JH, von Keyserlingk MA, Weary DM (2013) Pain and pessimism: dairy calves exhibit negative judgement bias following hot-iron disbudding. PLoS One 8, e80556
Pain and pessimism: dairy calves exhibit negative judgement bias following hot-iron disbudding.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 24324609PubMed |

Okada K, Takemura K, Sato S (2013) Investigation of various essential factors for optimum infrared thermography. The Journal of Veterinary Medical Science 75, 1349–1353.
Investigation of various essential factors for optimum infrared thermography.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 23759714PubMed |

Parola F, Hillmann E, Schütz KE, Tucker CB (2012) Preferences for overhead sprinklers by naïve beef steers: test of two nozzle types. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 137, 13–22.
Preferences for overhead sprinklers by naïve beef steers: test of two nozzle types.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Perkins N (2013) Animal health survey of the Australian feedlot industry (2010). MLA final report P.PSH.0547, Meat and Livestock Australia, Sydney, NSW, Australia.

Petherick JC (2005) Animal welfare issues associated with extensive livestock production: the northern Australian beef cattle industry. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 92, 211–234.
Animal welfare issues associated with extensive livestock production: the northern Australian beef cattle industry.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Petherick JC, Holroyd RG, Doogan VJ, Venus BK (2002) Productivity, carcass and meat quality of lot-fed Bos indicus cross steers grouped according to temperament. Australian Journal of Experimental Agriculture 42, 389–398.
Productivity, carcass and meat quality of lot-fed Bos indicus cross steers grouped according to temperament.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Petherick JC, Doogan VJ, Holroyd RG, Olsson P, Venus BK (2009) Quality of handling and holding yard environment, and beef cattle temperament: 1. Relationships with flight speed and fear of humans. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 120, 18–27.
Quality of handling and holding yard environment, and beef cattle temperament: 1. Relationships with flight speed and fear of humans.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Phillips C, Wojciechowska J, Meng J, Cross N (2009) Perceptions of the importance of different welfare issues in livestock production. Animal 3, 1152–1166.
Perceptions of the importance of different welfare issues in livestock production.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 22444845PubMed |

Proctor HS, Carder G (2015) Measuring positive emotions in cows: do visible eye whites tell us anything? Physiology and Behavior 147, 1–6.
Measuring positive emotions in cows: do visible eye whites tell us anything?Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 25862928PubMed |

Proctor H, Carder G (2016) Can changes in nasal temperature be used as an indicator of emotional state in cows? Applied Animal Behaviour Science 184, 1–6.
Can changes in nasal temperature be used as an indicator of emotional state in cows?Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Putz AM, Harding JC, Dyck MK, Fortin F, Plastow GS, Dekkers JC, Canada P (2019) Novel resilience phenotypes using feed intake data from a natural disease challenge model in wean-to-finish pigs. Frontiers in Genetics 9, 660
Novel resilience phenotypes using feed intake data from a natural disease challenge model in wean-to-finish pigs.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 30671080PubMed |

Rademacher RD, Warr BN, Booker CW (2015) Management of pregnant heifers in the feedlot. The Veterinary Clinics of North America. Food Animal Practice 31, 209–228.
Management of pregnant heifers in the feedlot.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 26139188PubMed |

Rahman A, Smith DV, Little B, Ingham AB, Greenwood PL, Bishop-Hurley GJ (2018) Cattle behaviour classification from collar, halter, and ear tag sensors. Information Processing in Agriculture 5, 124–133.
Cattle behaviour classification from collar, halter, and ear tag sensors.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Redbo I (1992) The influence of restraint on the occurrence of oral stereotypies in dairy cows. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 35, 115–123.
The influence of restraint on the occurrence of oral stereotypies in dairy cows.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Reinhardt C, Hands M, Marston T, Waggoner J, Corah L (2012) Relationships between feedlot health, average daily gain, and carcass traits of Angus steers. The Professional Animal Scientist 28, 11–19.
Relationships between feedlot health, average daily gain, and carcass traits of Angus steers.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Rezac DJ, Thomson DU, Bartle SJ, Osterstock JB, Prouty FL, Reinhardt CD (2014) Prevalence, severity, and relationships of lung lesions, liver abnormalities, and rumen health scores measured at slaughter in beef cattle. Journal of Animal Science 92, 2595–2602.
Prevalence, severity, and relationships of lung lesions, liver abnormalities, and rumen health scores measured at slaughter in beef cattle.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 24753377PubMed |

Ring SC, McCarthy J, Kelleher MM, Doherty ML, Berry DP (2018) Risk factors associated with animal mortality in pasture-based, seasonal-calving dairy and beef herds. Journal of Animal Science 96, 35–55.
Risk factors associated with animal mortality in pasture-based, seasonal-calving dairy and beef herds.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 29385481PubMed |

Rizhova LY, Kokorina EP (2005) Behavioral asymmetry is involved in regulation of autonomic processes: left side presentation of food improves reproduction and lactation in cows. Behavioural Brain Research 161, 75–81.
Behavioral asymmetry is involved in regulation of autonomic processes: left side presentation of food improves reproduction and lactation in cows.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 15904712PubMed |

Robertshaw D (1985) Heat loss of cattle. In ‘Stress physiology in livestock no. I’. (Ed. MK Yousef) pp. 55–66. (CRC Press: Boca Raton, FL, USA)

Robins A, Phillips C (2010) Lateralised visual processing in domestic cattle herds responding to novel and familiar stimuli. Laterality: asymmetries of body. Brain and Cognition 15, 514–534.

Robins A, Goma AA, Ouine L, Phillips CJ (2018) The eyes have it: lateralized coping strategies in cattle herds responding to human approach. Animal Cognition 21, 685–702.
The eyes have it: lateralized coping strategies in cattle herds responding to human approach.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 30006794PubMed |

Rogers LJ (2010) Relevance of brain and behavioral lateralization to animal welfare. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 127, 1–11.
Relevance of brain and behavioral lateralization to animal welfare.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Rushen J, de Passillé AMB, Munksgaard L (1999) Fear of people by cows and effects on milk yield, behavior, and heart rate at milking. Journal of Dairy Science 82, 720–727.
Fear of people by cows and effects on milk yield, behavior, and heart rate at milking.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 10212458PubMed |

Rutter SM (2010) Review. Grazing preferences in sheep and cattle: implications for production, the environment and animal welfare. Canadian Journal of Animal Science 90, 285–293.
Review. Grazing preferences in sheep and cattle: implications for production, the environment and animal welfare.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Sackett D, Holmes P, Abbot K, Jephcott S, Barber M (2006) Assessing the economic cost of endemic disease on the profitability of Australian beef cattle and sheep producers. MLA Final Report AHW.087, Meat and Livestock Australia, Sydney, NSW, Australia.

Sandem A-I, Braastad BO, Bøe KE (2002) Eye white may indicate emotional state on a frustration–contentedness axis in dairy cows. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 79, 1–10.
Eye white may indicate emotional state on a frustration–contentedness axis in dairy cows.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Sandem A-I, Braastad BO, Bakken M (2006) Behavior and percentage eye-white in cows waiting to be fed concentrate: a brief report. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 97, 145–151.
Behavior and percentage eye-white in cows waiting to be fed concentrate: a brief report.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Sanger ME, Doyle RE, Hinch GN, Lee C (2011) Sheep exhibit a positive judgement bias and stress-induced hyperthermia following shearing. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 131, 94–103.
Sheep exhibit a positive judgement bias and stress-induced hyperthermia following shearing.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Sant’Anna AC, Paranhos da Costa MJR (2013) Validity and feasibility of qualitative behavior assessment for the evaluation of Nellore cattle temperament. Livestock Science 157, 254–262.
Validity and feasibility of qualitative behavior assessment for the evaluation of Nellore cattle temperament.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Šárová R, Gutmann AK, Špinka M, Stěhulová I, Winckler C (2016) Important role of dominance in allogrooming behavior in beef cattle. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 181, 41–48.
Important role of dominance in allogrooming behavior in beef cattle.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Sato S, Sako S, Maeda A (1991) Social licking patterns in cattle (Bos taurus): influence of environmental and social factors. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 32, 3–12.
Social licking patterns in cattle (Bos taurus): influence of environmental and social factors.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Sato S, Tarumizu K, Hatae K (1993) The influence of social factors on allogrooming in cows. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 38, 235–244.
The influence of social factors on allogrooming in cows.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Sato S, Ueno N, Seo T, Tokumoto K (1994) Haloperidol injections entirely suppress tongue-playing in cattle. Journal of Ethology 12, 77–80.
Haloperidol injections entirely suppress tongue-playing in cattle.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Scheffer M, Bolhuis JE, Borsboom D, Buchman TG, Gijzel SM, Goulson D, Kammenga JE, Kemp B, van de Leemput IA, Levin S (2018) Quantifying resilience of humans and other animals. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 115, 11883–11890.
Quantifying resilience of humans and other animals.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 30373844PubMed |

Schütz KE, Rogers AR, Cox NR, Webster JR, Tucker CB (2011) Dairy cattle prefer shade over sprinklers: effects on behavior and physiology. Journal of Dairy Science 94, 273–283.
Dairy cattle prefer shade over sprinklers: effects on behavior and physiology.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 21183037PubMed |

Schütz KE, Lee C, DeVries TJ (2018) Cattle priorities. Feed and water selection, ability to move freely and to access pasture. In ‘Advances in cattle welfare.’ (Ed. CB Tucker) pp. 93–122. (Woodhead Publishing, Duxford, UK)

Seo T, Sato S, Kosaka K, Sakamoto N, Tokumoto K (1998) Tongue-playing and heart rate in calves. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 58, 179–182.
Tongue-playing and heart rate in calves.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Sih A, Christensen B (2001) Optimal diet theory: when does it work, and when and why does it fail? Animal Behaviour 61, 379–390.
Optimal diet theory: when does it work, and when and why does it fail?Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Simon GE, Hoar BR, Tucker CB (2016) Assessing cow–calf welfare. Part 1: benchmarking beef cow health and behavior, handling; and management, facilities, and producer perspectives. Journal of Animal Science 94, 3476–3487.
Assessing cow–calf welfare. Part 1: benchmarking beef cow health and behavior, handling; and management, facilities, and producer perspectives.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 27695797PubMed |

Snowder GD, Van Vleck LD, Cundiff LV, Bennett GL (2006) Bovine respiratory disease in feedlot cattle: environmental, genetic, and economic factors. Journal of Animal Science 84, 1999–2008.
Bovine respiratory disease in feedlot cattle: environmental, genetic, and economic factors.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 16864858PubMed |

Sowell BF, Bowman JG, Huisma ME, Branine ME, Hubbert ME (1997). Feeding behavior of feedlot cattle. In ‘Proceedings of the 12th South-west nutrition and management’ pp. 45-49. (Phoenix, AZ, USA)

Spain C, Freund D, Mohan-Gibbons H, Meadow R, Beacham L (2018) Are they buying It? United States consumers’ changing attitudes toward more humanely raised meat, eggs, and dairy. Animals 8, 128
Are they buying It? United States consumers’ changing attitudes toward more humanely raised meat, eggs, and dairy.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Stewart M, Schaefer A, Haley D, Colyn J, Cook N, Stafford K, Webster J (2008) Infrared thermography as a non-invasive method for detecting fear-related responses of cattle to handling procedures. Animal Welfare 17, 387–393.

Stewart M, Verkerk GA, Stafford KJ, Schaefer AL, Webster JR (2010a) Noninvasive assessment of autonomic activity for evaluation of pain in calves, using surgical castration as a model. Journal of Dairy Science 93, 3602–3609.
Noninvasive assessment of autonomic activity for evaluation of pain in calves, using surgical castration as a model.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 20655429PubMed |

Stewart M, Webster JR, Stafford KJ, Schaefer AL, Verkerk GA (2010b) Technical note: effects of an epinephrine infusion on eye temperature and heart rate variability in bull calves. Journal of Dairy Science 93, 5252–5257.
Technical note: effects of an epinephrine infusion on eye temperature and heart rate variability in bull calves.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 20965341PubMed |

Stockman CA, Collins T, Barnes AL, Miller D, Wickham SL, Beatty DT, Blache D, Wemelsfelder F, Fleming PA (2011) Qualitative behavioural assessment and quantitative physiological measurement of cattle naïve and habituated to road transport. Animal Production Science 51, 240–249.
Qualitative behavioural assessment and quantitative physiological measurement of cattle naïve and habituated to road transport.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Stockman CA, McGilchrist P, Collins T, Barnes AL, Miller DW, Wickham SL, Greenwood PL, Cafe LM, Blache D, Wemelsfelder F, Fleming PA (2012) Qualitative behavioural assessment of cattle pre-slaughter and relationship with cattle temperament and physiological responses to the slaughter process. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 142, 125–133.
Qualitative behavioural assessment of cattle pre-slaughter and relationship with cattle temperament and physiological responses to the slaughter process.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Stockman CA, Collins T, Barnes AL, Miller DW, Wickham SL, Beatty DT, Blache D, Wemelsfelder F, Fleming PA (2013) Flooring and driving conditions during road transport influence the behavioural expression of cattle. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 143, 18–30.
Flooring and driving conditions during road transport influence the behavioural expression of cattle.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Sullivan ML, Cawdell-Smith AJ, Mader TL, Gaughan JB (2011) Effect of shade area on performance and welfare of short-fed feedlot cattle. Journal of Animal Science 89, 2911–2925.
Effect of shade area on performance and welfare of short-fed feedlot cattle.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 21478450PubMed |

Sumner CL, von Keyserlingk MAG, Weary DM (2018) How benchmarking motivates farmers to improve dairy calf management. Journal of Dairy Science 101, 3323–3333.
How benchmarking motivates farmers to improve dairy calf management.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 29397181PubMed |

Tallo-Parra O, Manteca X, Sabes-Alsina M, Carbajal A, Lopez-Bejar M (2015) Hair cortisol detection in dairy cattle by using EIA: protocol validation and correlation with faecal cortisol metabolites Animal 9, 1059–1064.
Hair cortisol detection in dairy cattle by using EIA: protocol validation and correlation with faecal cortisol metabolitesCrossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 25997530PubMed |

Tennessen T, Price MA, Berg RT (1985) The social interactions of young bulls and steers after re-grouping. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 14, 37–47.
The social interactions of young bulls and steers after re-grouping.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Terrell S, Thomson D, Wileman B, Apley M (2011) A survey to describe current feeder cattle health and well-being program recommendations made by feedlot veterinary consultants in the United States and Canada. The Bovine Practitioner 45, 140–148.

Thomsen PT, Houe H (2018) Cow mortality as an indicator of animal welfare in dairy herds. Research in Veterinary Science 119, 239–243.
Cow mortality as an indicator of animal welfare in dairy herds.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 30005399PubMed |

Tilman D, Cassman KG, Matson PA, Naylor R, Polasky S (2002) Agricultural sustainability and intensive production practices. Nature 418, 671–677.
Agricultural sustainability and intensive production practices.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 12167873PubMed |

Tresoldi G, Schütz KE, Tucker CB (2018) Cooling cows with sprinklers: spray duration affects physiological responses to heat load. Journal of Dairy Science 101, 4412–4423.
Cooling cows with sprinklers: spray duration affects physiological responses to heat load.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 29501342PubMed |

Tucker CB, Coetzee JF, Stookey JM, Thomson DU, Grandin T, Schwartzkopf-Genswein KS (2015) Beef cattle welfare in the USA: identification of priorities for future research. Animal Health Research Reviews 16, 107–124.
Beef cattle welfare in the USA: identification of priorities for future research.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 26459152PubMed |

Tucker CB, Munksgaard L, Mintline EM, Jensen MB (2018) Use of a pneumatic push gate to measure dairy cattle motivation to lie down in a deep-bedded area. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 201, 15–24.
Use of a pneumatic push gate to measure dairy cattle motivation to lie down in a deep-bedded area.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Turner SP, Navajas EA, Hyslop JJ, Ross DW, Richardson RI, Prieto N, Bell M, Jack MC, Roehe R (2011) Associations between response to handling and growth and meat quality in frequently handled Bos taurus beef cattle. Journal of Animal Science 89, 4239–4248.
Associations between response to handling and growth and meat quality in frequently handled Bos taurus beef cattle.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 21841078PubMed |

Val-Laillet D, Guesdon V, von Keyserlingk MAG, de Passillé AM, Rushen J (2009) Allogrooming in cattle: relationships between social preferences, feeding displacements and social dominance. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 116, 141–149.
Allogrooming in cattle: relationships between social preferences, feeding displacements and social dominance.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Van Os JMC, Mintline EM, DeVries TJ, Tucker CB (2017a) Feedlot cattle are motivated to obtain roughage and show contrafreeloading. p. 59. In ‘Proceedings of the 51st congress of the International Society for Applied Ethology’. (Eds MB Jensen, MS Herskin, J Malmkvist) (Wageningen Academic Publishers: Wageningen, Netherlands)

Van Os JMC, Mintline EM, DeVries TJ, Tucker CB (2017b) Motivation of naïve feedlot cattle to obtain grain and individual responses to novelty. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 197, 68–74.
Motivation of naïve feedlot cattle to obtain grain and individual responses to novelty.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Van Reenen CG, O’Connell NE, Van der Werf JTN, Korte SM, Hopster H, Jones RB, Blokhuis HJ (2005) Responses of calves to acute stress: individual consistency and relations between behavioral and physiological measures. Physiology and Behavior 85, 557–570.
Responses of calves to acute stress: individual consistency and relations between behavioral and physiological measures.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 16081113PubMed |

Vandenheede M, Nicks B, Shehi R, Canart B, Dufrasne I, Biston R, Lecomte P (1995) Use of a shelter by grazing fattening bulls: effect of climatic factors. Animal Science 60, 81–85.
Use of a shelter by grazing fattening bulls: effect of climatic factors.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Vanhonacker F, Verbeke W, Van Poucke E, Tuyttens FAM (2008) Do citizens and farmers interpret the concept of farm animal welfare differently? Livestock Science 116, 126–136.
Do citizens and farmers interpret the concept of farm animal welfare differently?Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Verbeek E, Ferguson D, Quinquet de Monjour P, Lee C (2014) Generating positive affective states in sheep: the influence of food rewards and opioid administration. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 154, 39–47.
Generating positive affective states in sheep: the influence of food rewards and opioid administration.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Vickers LA, Burfeind O, von Keyserlingk MAG, Veira DM, Weary DM, Heuwieser W (2010) Technical note: comparison of rectal and vaginal temperatures in lactating dairy cows. Journal of Dairy Science 93, 5246–5251.
Technical note: comparison of rectal and vaginal temperatures in lactating dairy cows.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 20965340PubMed |

Vinkers CH, Groenink L, van Bogaert MJV, Westphal KGC, Kalkman CJ, van Oorschot R, Oosting RS, Olivier B, Korte SM (2009) Stress-induced hyperthermia and infection-induced fever: two of a kind? Physiology and Behavior 98, 37–43.
Stress-induced hyperthermia and infection-induced fever: two of a kind?Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 19375439PubMed |

Vogel GJ, Bokenkroger CD, Rutten-Ramos SC, Bargen JL (2015) A retrospective evaluation of animal mortality in US feedlots: rate, timing, and cause of death. The Bovine Practitioner 49, 113–123.

Voisinet BD, Grandin T, Tatum JD, O’Connor SF, Struthers JJ (1997a) Feedlot cattle with calm temperaments have higher average daily gains than cattle with excitable temperaments. Journal of Animal Science 75, 892–896.
Feedlot cattle with calm temperaments have higher average daily gains than cattle with excitable temperaments.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 9110198PubMed |

Voisinet BD, Grandin T, O’Connor SF, Tatum JD, Deesing MJ (1997b) Bos indicus-cross feedlot cattle with excitable temperaments have tougher meat and a higher incidence of borderline dark cutters. Meat Science 46, 367–377.
Bos indicus-cross feedlot cattle with excitable temperaments have tougher meat and a higher incidence of borderline dark cutters.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 22062320PubMed |

Von Keyserlingk MA, Cestari AA, Franks B, Fregonesi JA, Weary DM (2017) Dairy cows value access to pasture as highly as fresh feed. Scientific Reports 7, 44953
Dairy cows value access to pasture as highly as fresh feed.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 28332567PubMed |

Waiblinger S, Menke C, Coleman G (2002) The relationship between attitudes, personal characteristics and behavior of stockpeople and subsequent behavior and production of dairy cows. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 79, 195–219.
The relationship between attitudes, personal characteristics and behavior of stockpeople and subsequent behavior and production of dairy cows.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Waiblinger S, Boivin X, Pedersen V, Tosi M-V, Janczak AM, Visser EK, Jones RB (2006) Assessing the human–animal relationship in farmed species: a critical review. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 101, 185–242.
Assessing the human–animal relationship in farmed species: a critical review.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Walker KH, Fell LR, Reddacliff LA, Kilgour RJ, House JR, Wilson SC, Nicholls PJ (2007) Effects of yard weaning and training on the behavioral adaptation of cattle to a feedlot. Livestock Science 106, 210–217.
Effects of yard weaning and training on the behavioral adaptation of cattle to a feedlot.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Watts PJ, Yan MJ, Sullivan TJ, Luttrell MM, Davis RJ, Keane OB (2015) Feedlot bedding study. MLA Final Report B.FLT.0237, Meat and Livestock Australia, Sydney, NSW, Australia.

Watts PJ, Davis RJ, Keane OB, Luttrell MM, Tucker RW, Stafford R, Janke S (2016). ‘Beef cattle feedlots: design and construction.’ (Meat and Livestock Australia: Sydney, NSW, Australia)

Weary D, Huzzey JM, Keyserlingk M (2009) Board-invited rview: using behavior to predict and identify ill health in animals. Journal of Animal Science 87, 770–777.
Board-invited rview: using behavior to predict and identify ill health in animals.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 18952731PubMed |

Welfare Quality (2009) ‘Welfare Quality® assessment protocol for cattle.’ Available at http://www.welfarequalitynetwork.net/en-us/reports/assessment-protocols/. [Verified 7 April 2020]

Welp T, Rushen J, Kramer DL, Festa-Bianchet M, de Passille AM (2004) Vigilance as a measure of fear in dairy cattle. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 87, 1–13.
Vigilance as a measure of fear in dairy cattle.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Williams J, Randle H (2017) Is the expression of stereotypic behavior a performance-limiting factor in animals? Journal of Veterinary Behavior: Clinical Applications and Research 20, 1–10.
Is the expression of stereotypic behavior a performance-limiting factor in animals?Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Wilson SC, Fell LR, Colditz IG, Collins DP (2002a) An examination of some physiological variables for assessing the welfare of beef cattle in feedlots. Animal Welfare 11, 305–316.

Wilson SC, Mitlöhner FM, Morrow-Tesch J, Dailey JW, McGlone JJ (2002b) An assessment of several potential enrichment devices for feedlot cattle. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 76, 259–265.
An assessment of several potential enrichment devices for feedlot cattle.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Wilson SC, Dobos RC, Fell LR (2005) Spectral analysis of feeding and lying behavior of cattle kept under different feedlot conditions. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 8, 13–24.
Spectral analysis of feeding and lying behavior of cattle kept under different feedlot conditions.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 16004542PubMed |

Winckler C, Capdeville J, Gebresenbet G, Hörning B, Roiha U, Tosi M, Waiblinger S (2003) Selection of parameters for on-farm welfare-assessment protocols in cattle and buffalo. Animal Welfare 12, 619–624.

Windschnurer I, Boivin X, Waiblinger S (2009) Reliability of an avoidance distance test for the assessment of animals’ responsiveness to humans and a preliminary investigation of its association with farmers’ attitudes on bull fattening farms. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 117, 117–127.
Reliability of an avoidance distance test for the assessment of animals’ responsiveness to humans and a preliminary investigation of its association with farmers’ attitudes on bull fattening farms.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Woiwode R, Grandin T, Kirch B, Paterson J (2016) Compliance of large feedyards in the northern high plains with the Beef Quality Assurance Feedyard Assessment. The Professional Animal Scientist 32, 750–757.
Compliance of large feedyards in the northern high plains with the Beef Quality Assurance Feedyard Assessment.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Young BA, Hall AB (1993) Heat load in cattle in the Australian environment. In ‘Australian Beef’. (Ed. R Coombes) pp. 143–148. (Morescope Publishing: Melbourne, Vic., Australia.)

Yousef MK (1985) Thermal environment. In ‘Stress physiology in livestock no. I’. (Ed. MK Yousef) pp. 9–13. (CRC Press: Boca Raton, FL, USA)




1 http://www.feedlots.com.au [Verified 24 June 2020].

2 http://chlt.katestone.com.au/. [Verified 24 June 2020].

3 https://www.allflex.global/ [Verified 24 June 2020]; http://www.icerobotics.com/ [Verified 24 June 2020]; https://www.dairymaster.com/products/moomonitor/ [Verified 24 June 2020].