Free Standard AU & NZ Shipping For All Book Orders Over $80!
Register      Login
Marine and Freshwater Research Marine and Freshwater Research Society
Advances in the aquatic sciences
RESEARCH ARTICLE

Friend or foe? Development of odour detection, differentiation and antipredator response in an embryonic elasmobranch

Connor R. Gervais https://orcid.org/0000-0001-6501-9798 A C , Tiffany Nay https://orcid.org/0000-0002-2410-5961 B and Culum Brown https://orcid.org/0000-0002-0210-1820 A
+ Author Affiliations
- Author Affiliations

A Department of Biological Sciences, Macquarie University, Sydney, NSW 2109, Australia.

B ARC Centre of Excellence for Coral Reef Studies, James Cook University, James Cook Drive, Townsville, Qld 4811, Australia.

C Corresponding author. Email: cgervais9249@gmail.com

Marine and Freshwater Research 72(7) 942-949 https://doi.org/10.1071/MF20108
Submitted: 11 April 2020  Accepted: 20 November 2020   Published: 2 February 2021

Abstract

Young animals, especially those developing within eggs, are extremely vulnerable to predation risk given their immobility, small size and limited functionality of developing sensory systems. Embryos from a range of taxa can detect predator cues and use antipredator responses to reduce risk; however, little is known about this capacity in elasmobranchs, especially regarding olfactory detection. Embryonic Port Jackson sharks (Heterodontus portusjacksoni) were exposed to elasmobranch and teleost odours across key developmental stages to investigate their capacity to detect and discern between cues. Oxygen uptake rates were measured as a proxy for antipredator response and to estimate their energetic costs. Earlier developmental stages exhibited limited responses, regardless of the odour, likely associated with an undeveloped sensory system. However, older shark embryos depressed oxygen uptake rates (i.e. crypsis responses) when exposed to teleost cues, but showed little response to elasmobranch cues. In contrast, hatchlings exhibited minimal responses to teleost cues but a significant increase in oxygen uptake rates when exposed to elasmobranch odours, indicative of a general stress response in preparation for escape. Collectively, our data suggest that embryonic sharks can differentiate between odour cues and elicit various responses, but this ability is limited by their developing sensory capacity.

Keywords: cognition, Heterodontus, olfaction, oxygen uptake, physiology, predation.


References

Atherton, J. A., and McCormick, M. I. (2015). Active in the sac: damselfish embryos use innate recognition of odours to learn predation risk before hatching. Animal Behaviour 103, 1–6.
Active in the sac: damselfish embryos use innate recognition of odours to learn predation risk before hatching.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Ball, R. E., Oliver, M. K., and Gill, A. B. (2016). Early life sensory ability – ventilatory responses of thornback ray embryos (Raja clavata) to predator-type electric fields: electroreception in Rajid embryos. Developmental Neurobiology 76, 721–729.
Early life sensory ability – ventilatory responses of thornback ray embryos (Raja clavata) to predator-type electric fields: electroreception in Rajid embryos.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 26468062PubMed |

Bedore, C. N., Kajiura, S. M., and Johnsen, S. (2015). Freezing behaviour facilitates bioelectric crypsis in cuttlefish faced with predation risk. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London – B. Biological Sciences 282, 20151886.
Freezing behaviour facilitates bioelectric crypsis in cuttlefish faced with predation risk.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Bor, P. H. F., and Santos, M. B. (2003). Findings of elasmobranch eggs in the stomachs of sperm whales and other marine organisms. Journal of the Marine Biological Association of the United Kingdom 83, 1351–1353.
Findings of elasmobranch eggs in the stomachs of sperm whales and other marine organisms.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Briceño, F. A., Polymeropoulos, E. T., Fitzgibbon, Q. P., Dambacher, J. M., and Pecl, G. T. (2018). Changes in metabolic rate of spiny lobster under predation risk. Marine Ecology Progress Series 598, 71–84.
Changes in metabolic rate of spiny lobster under predation risk.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Brown, C., Krause, J. D., and Laland, K. N. (2011). ‘Fish Cognition and Behavior’, 2nd edn. (Wiley-Blackwell: Chichester, UK.)

Chabot, D., Steffensen, J. F., and Farrell, A. P. (2016). The determination of standard metabolic rate in fishes. Journal of Fish Biology 88, 81–121.
The determination of standard metabolic rate in fishes.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 26768973PubMed |

Chivers, D. P., and Smith, R. J. F. (1998). Chemical alarm signalling in aquatic predator-prey systems: a review and prospectus. Ecoscience 5, 338–352.
Chemical alarm signalling in aquatic predator-prey systems: a review and prospectus.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Clark, T. D., Sandblom, E., and Jutfelt, F. (2013). Aerobic scope measurements of fishes in an era of climate change: respirometry, relevance and recommendations. The Journal of Experimental Biology 216, 2771–2782.
Aerobic scope measurements of fishes in an era of climate change: respirometry, relevance and recommendations.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 23842625PubMed |

Conrath, C. L., and Musick, J. A. (2012). Reproductive biology of elasmobranchs. In ‘Biology of Sharks and Their Relatives’, 2 edn. (Eds J. C. Carrier, J. A. Musick, and M. R. Heithaus.) pp. 291–312. (CRC Press: Boca Raton, FL, USA.)

Cooke, S. J., Steinmetz, J., Degner, J. F., Grant, E. C., and Philipp, D. P. (2003). Metabolic fright responses of different-sized largemouth bass (Micropterus salmoides) to two avian predators show variations in nonlethal energetic costs. Canadian Journal of Zoology 81, 699–709.
Metabolic fright responses of different-sized largemouth bass (Micropterus salmoides) to two avian predators show variations in nonlethal energetic costs.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Cox, D. L., and Koob, T. J. (1993). Predation on elasmobranch eggs. Environmental Biology of Fishes 38, 117–125.
Predation on elasmobranch eggs.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Creel, S. (2018). The control of risk hypothesis: reactive vs. proactive antipredator responses and stress-mediated vs. food-mediated costs of response. Ecology Letters 21, 947–956.
The control of risk hypothesis: reactive vs. proactive antipredator responses and stress-mediated vs. food-mediated costs of response.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 29744982PubMed |

Endler, J. (1991). Interactions between predators and prey. In ‘Behavioural Ecology: An Evolutionary Approach’. (Ed. J. Endler.) pp. 169–196. (Blackwell Publishing: Oxford, UK.)

Ferrari, M. C. O., and Chivers, D. (2006). Learning threat-sensitive predator avoidance: how do fathead minnows incorporate conflicting information? Animal Behaviour 71, 19–26.
Learning threat-sensitive predator avoidance: how do fathead minnows incorporate conflicting information?Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Ferrari, M. C. O., and Chivers, D. P. (2009). Sophisticated early life lessons: threat-sensitive generalization of predator recognition by embryonic amphibians. Behavioral Ecology 20, 1295–1298.
Sophisticated early life lessons: threat-sensitive generalization of predator recognition by embryonic amphibians.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Ferrari, M. C. O., and Chivers, D. P. (2011). Learning about non-predators and safe places: the forgotten elements of risk assessment. Animal Cognition 14, 309–316.
Learning about non-predators and safe places: the forgotten elements of risk assessment.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Ferrari, M. C. O., Messier, F., and Chivers Douglas, P. (2008). Can prey exhibit threat-sensitive generalization of predator recognition? Extending the Predator Recognition Continuum Hypothesis. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London – B. Biological Sciences 275, 1811–1816.
Can prey exhibit threat-sensitive generalization of predator recognition? Extending the Predator Recognition Continuum Hypothesis.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Finucci, B., Bustamante, C., Jones, E. G., and Dunn, M. R. (2016). Reproductive biology and feeding habits of the prickly dogfish Oxynotus bruniensis. Journal of Fish Biology 89, 2326–2344.
Reproductive biology and feeding habits of the prickly dogfish Oxynotus bruniensis.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 27606943PubMed |

Fuiman, L. A., and Magurran, A. E. (1994). Development of predator defences in fishes. Reviews in Fish Biology and Fisheries 4, 145–183.
Development of predator defences in fishes.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Gardiner, J., Hueter, E. R., Maruska, K., Sisneros, J., Casper, B., Mann, D., and Demski, S. L. (2012). Sensory physiology and behavior of elasmobranchs. In ‘Biology of Sharks and Their Relatives’, 2 edn. (Eds J. C. Carrier, J. A. Musick, and M. R. Heithaus.) pp. 349–401. (CRC Press: Boca Raton, FL, USA.)

Guallart, J., García‐Salinas, P., Ahuir‐Baraja, A. E., Guimerans, M., Ellis, J. R., and Roche, M. (2015). Angular roughshark Oxynotus centrina (Squaliformes: Oxynotidae) in captivity feeding exclusively on elasmobranch eggs: an overlooked feeding niche or a matter of individual taste? Journal of Fish Biology 87, 1072–1079.
Angular roughshark Oxynotus centrina (Squaliformes: Oxynotidae) in captivity feeding exclusively on elasmobranch eggs: an overlooked feeding niche or a matter of individual taste?Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 26283180PubMed |

Hadwen, W. L., Russell, G. L., and Arthington, A. H. (2007). Gut content- and stable isotope-derived diets of four commercially and recreationally important fish species in two intermittently open estuaries. Marine and Freshwater Research 58, 363–375.
Gut content- and stable isotope-derived diets of four commercially and recreationally important fish species in two intermittently open estuaries.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Hamlett, W. C. (2005). ‘Reproductive Biology and Phylogeny of Chondrichthyes.’ (Science Publishers: Portland, OR, USA.)

Hawlena, D., and Schmitz, O. (2010). Physiological stress as a fundamental mechanism linking predation to ccosystem functioning. American Naturalist 176, 537–556.
Physiological stress as a fundamental mechanism linking predation to ccosystem functioning.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Hirvonen, H., Ranta, E., Piironen, J., Laurila, A., and Peuhkuri, N. (2000). Behavioural responses of naive Arctic charr young to chemical cues from salmonid and non-salmonid fish. Oikos 88, 191–199.
Behavioural responses of naive Arctic charr young to chemical cues from salmonid and non-salmonid fish.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Hoff, G. R. (2008). A nursery site of the Alaska skate (Bathyraja parmifera) in the eastern Bering Sea. Fishery Bulletin 106, 233–244.

Kempster, R. M., Hart, N. S., and Collin, S. P. (2013). Survival of the stillest: predator avoidance in shark embryos. PLoS One 8, e52551.
Survival of the stillest: predator avoidance in shark embryos.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 23326342PubMed |

Luer, C. A., and Gilbert, P. W. (1985). Mating behavior, egg deposition, incubation period, and hatching in the clearnose skate, Raja eglanteria. Environmental Biology of Fishes 13, 161–171.
Mating behavior, egg deposition, incubation period, and hatching in the clearnose skate, Raja eglanteria.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

McLaughlin, R., and O’Gower, A. (1971). Life history and underwater studies of a heterodont shark. Ecological Monographs 41, 271–289.
Life history and underwater studies of a heterodont shark.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Mezrai, N., Arduini, L., Dickel, L., Chiao, C.-C., and Darmaillacq, A.-S. (2020). Awareness of danger inside the egg: evidence of innate and learned predator recognition in cuttlefish embryos. Learning & Behavior 48, 401–410.
Awareness of danger inside the egg: evidence of innate and learned predator recognition in cuttlefish embryos.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Moore, R. D., Newton, B., and Sih, A. (1996). Delayed hatching as a response of streamside salamander eggs to chemical cues from predatory sunfish. Oikos 77, 331–335.
Delayed hatching as a response of streamside salamander eggs to chemical cues from predatory sunfish.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

O’Gower, A., and Nash, A. (1978). Dispersion of the Port Jackson shark in Australian waters. In ‘Sensory Biology of Sharks, Skates and Rays’. (Eds E. S. Hodgson and R. F. Mathewson.) pp. 529–544. (Office of Naval Research: Washington, DC, USA.)

Orians, G. H., and Janzen, D. H. (1974). Why are embryos so tasty? American Naturalist 108, 581–592.
Why are embryos so tasty?Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Oulton, L. J., Haviland, V., and Brown, C. (2013). Predator recognition in rainbowfish, Melanotaenia duboulayi, embryos. PLoS One 8, e76061.
Predator recognition in rainbowfish, Melanotaenia duboulayi, embryos.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 24146817PubMed |

Palacios, M. M., Killen, S. S., Nadler, L. E., White, J. R., and McCormick, M. I. (2016). Top predators negate the effect of mesopredators on prey physiology. Journal of Animal Ecology 85, 1078–1086.
Top predators negate the effect of mesopredators on prey physiology.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Powter, D. M., and Gladstone, W. (2008). Embryonic mortality and predation on egg capsules of the Port Jackson shark Heterodontus portusjacksoni (Meyer). Journal of Fish Biology 72, 573–584.
Embryonic mortality and predation on egg capsules of the Port Jackson shark Heterodontus portusjacksoni (Meyer).Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Rasmussen, L. E. L., and Schmidt, M. J. (1992). Are sharks chemically aware of crocodiles? In ‘Chemical Signals in Vertebrates 6’. (Eds R. L. Doty and D. Müller-Schwarze.) pp. 335–342. (Springer US: Boston, MA, USA.)

Rodda, K. R., and Seymour, R. S. (2008). Functional morphology of embryonic development in the Port Jackson shark Heterodontus portusjacksoni (Meyer). Journal of Fish Biology 72, 961–984.
Functional morphology of embryonic development in the Port Jackson shark Heterodontus portusjacksoni (Meyer).Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Sims, D. W. (2000). Can threshold foraging responses of basking sharks be used to estimate their metabolic rate? Marine Ecology Progress Series 200, 289–296.
Can threshold foraging responses of basking sharks be used to estimate their metabolic rate?Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Sisneros, J. A., Tricas, T. C., and Luer, C. A. (1998). Response properties and biological function of the skate electrosensory system during ontogeny. Journal of Comparative Physiology – A. Neuroethology, Sensory, Neural, and Behavioral Physiology 183, 87–99.
Response properties and biological function of the skate electrosensory system during ontogeny.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Smith, E. N., Sims, K., and Vich, J. F. (1981). Oxygen consumption of frightened swamp rabbits, Sylvilagus aquaticus. Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology – A. Physiology 70, 533–536.
Oxygen consumption of frightened swamp rabbits, Sylvilagus aquaticus.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Sneddon, L. U., Braithwaite, V. A., and Gentle, M. J. (2003). Novel object test: examining nociception and fear in the rainbow trout. The Journal of Pain 4, 431–440.
Novel object test: examining nociception and fear in the rainbow trout.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 14622663PubMed |

Steiner, U. K., and Van Buskirk, J. (2009). Predator-induced changes in metabolism cannot explain the growth/predation risk tradeoff. PLoS One 4, e6160.
Predator-induced changes in metabolism cannot explain the growth/predation risk tradeoff.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 19582147PubMed |

Stroud, E. M., O’Connell, C. P., Rice, P. H., Snow, N. H., Barnes, B. B., Elshaer, M. R., and Hanson, J. E. (2014). Chemical shark repellent: myth or fact? The effect of a shark necromone on shark feeding behavior. Ocean and Coastal Management 97, 50–57.
Chemical shark repellent: myth or fact? The effect of a shark necromone on shark feeding behavior.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Warkentin, K. (1995). Adaptive plasticity in hatching age: a response to predation risk trade-offs. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 92, 3507–3510.
Adaptive plasticity in hatching age: a response to predation risk trade-offs.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 11607529PubMed |

Warkentin, K. (2005). How do embryos assess risk? Vibrational cues in predator-induced hatching of red-eyed treefrogs. Animal Behaviour 70, 59–71.
How do embryos assess risk? Vibrational cues in predator-induced hatching of red-eyed treefrogs.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Wourms, J. P. (1977). Reproduction and development in chondrichthyan fishes. American Zoologist 17, 379–410.
Reproduction and development in chondrichthyan fishes.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Yao, M., Rosenfeld, J., Attridge, S., Sidhu, S., Aksenov, V., and Rollo, C. D. (2009). The ancient chemistry of avoiding risks of predation and disease. Evolutionary Biology 36, 267–281.
The ancient chemistry of avoiding risks of predation and disease.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |