Register      Login
Emu Emu Society
Journal of BirdLife Australia
RESEARCH ARTICLE

Stealing rates in the Great Bowerbird (Ptilonorhynchus nuchalis): effects of the spatial arrangement of males and availability of decorations

Natalie R. Doerr
+ Author Affiliations
- Author Affiliations

A School of Marine and Tropical Biology, James Cook University, Townsville, Qld 4811, Australia.

B Present address: Department of Ecology, Evolution, and Marine Biology, University of California, Santa Barbara, CA 93106, USA. Email: doerr@lifesci.ucsb.edu

Emu 109(3) 230-236 https://doi.org/10.1071/MU09004
Submitted: 25 January 2009  Accepted: 4 May 2009   Published: 17 August 2009

Abstract

Competitive interactions between males can affect mate-choice decisions of females, so it is important to understand the factors that underlie variation in the frequency and intensity of male–male interactions. In bowerbirds (Ptilonorhynchidae), two hypotheses have been proposed to explain within- and between-species variation in the rate at which males steal each other’s decorations. Males may steal more frequently as inter-bower distances decrease because this reduces the time and energy costs of travelling between bowers, or they may steal more frequently when bowers contain relatively few decorations, compared with bowers in other populations or species, because this leads to an increase in the value of decorations to males. I compared stealing rates in two populations of Great Bowerbirds (Ptilonorhynchus nuchalis) in Queensland, Australia, in order to assess these hypotheses. Males at one site, Dreghorn, had fewer decorations, higher nearest-neighbour distances (NND), and fewer males within a 1-km radius of their bowers (termed bower density) than males at the Townsville site. Dreghorn males stole decorations at a lower rate and interacted with fewer individuals, though there was no difference between sites in the rate at which males stole decorations from their nearest neighbours. Within sites, stealing rates were not related to decoration numbers, and partial correlations revealed that stealing rates were correlated with bower density, not NND. These results suggest that differences in the spatial arrangement of males, particularly bower density, may explain variation in stealing rates both within and between populations, though alternative explanations, such as differences in resource availability, are also relevant.


Acknowledgements

This study was supported with a National Science Foundation International Travel Grant, a National Science Foundation Graduate Research Fellowship, and a University of California Regents Special Fellowship. Thanks to Allan McManus and the Australian Army for permission to work at Lavarack Barracks; to the Smith family for permission to work at Dreghorn Station; and to Rhona Barr, Lainy Day and David Wilson for assistance in the field. John Endler, Stephen Pruett-Jones, and two anonymous referees provided helpful comments on the manuscript. This study was conducted with permission from Queensland Parks and Wildlife, the University of California Animal Care and Use Committee (IACUC), and the James Cook University Ethics Committee.


References

Berglund, A. , Bisazza, A. , and Pilastro, A. (1996). Armaments and ornaments: an evolutionary explanation of traits of dual utility. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society. Linnean Society of London 58, 385–399.
Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | Doerr N. R. (2008). Male-male competition and the cost of display in the Great Bowerbird (Chalmydera nuchalis). Ph.D. Thesis, University of California, Santa Barbara.

Frith C. B. , and Frith D. W. (2004). ‘The Bowerbirds: Ptilonorhynchidae.’ (Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK.)

Frith, C. B. , Frith, D. W. , and Wieneke, J. (1996). Dispersion, size and orientation of bowers of the Great Bowerbird Chlamydera nuchalis (Ptilonorhynchidae) in Townsville City, Tropical Queensland. Corella 20, 45–55.
Gibbons J. D. (1993). ‘Nonparametric Measures of Association.’ (Sage Publications: Newbury Park, Greater London, UK.)

Howard, R. D. , Moorman, R. S. , and Whiteman, H. H. (1997). Differential effects of mate competition and mate choice on eastern tiger salamanders. Animal Behaviour 53, 1345–1356.
Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | PubMed | Marshall A. J. (1954). ‘Bower-birds, Their Displays and Breeding Cycles – A Preliminary Statement.’ (Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK.)

Maxwell, P. S. , Robson, T. E. , and Goldizen, A. W. (2004). Home range sizes and bower visitation patterns of immature male Satin Bowerbirds (Ptilonorhynchus violaceus). Emu 104, 75–77.
Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | SPSS Inc. (2002). SPSS for Windows, Release 11.5.0. (SPSS Inc. Chicago, IL, USA.)

Vellenga, R. E. (1970). Behaviour of the male Satin Bower-Bird at the bower. Australian Bird Bander 8, 3–11.


Wojcieszek, J. M. , Nicholls, J. A. , Marshall, N. J. , and Goldizen, A. W. (2006). Theft of bower decorations among male Satin Bowerbirds (Ptilonorhynchus violaceus): why are some decorations more popular than others? Emu 106, 175–180.
Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Wojcieszek, J. M. , Nicholls, J. A. , and Goldizen, A. W. (2007). Stealing behavior and the maintenance of a visual display in the satin bowerbird. Behavioral Ecology 18, 689–695.
Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Wong, B. B. M. , and Candolin, U. (2005). How is female mate choice affected by male competition? Biological Reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society 80, 559–571.
Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | PubMed |