Free Standard AU & NZ Shipping For All Book Orders Over $80!
Register      Login
Marine and Freshwater Research Marine and Freshwater Research Society
Advances in the aquatic sciences
RESEARCH ARTICLE

Behavioural transition probabilities in dugongs change with habitat and predator presence: implications for sirenian conservation

Aaron J. Wirsing A C and Michael R. Heithaus B
+ Author Affiliations
- Author Affiliations

A School of Environmental and Forest Sciences, Box 352100, University of Washington, Seattle, WA 98195, USA.

B School of Environment, Arts and Society, Florida International University, ACI-318, 3000 NE 151st, North Miami, FL 33181, USA.

C Corresponding author. Email: wirsinga@u.washington.edu

Marine and Freshwater Research 63(11) 1069-1076 https://doi.org/10.1071/MF12074
Submitted: 11 March 2012  Accepted: 11 June 2012   Published: 26 November 2012

Abstract

Although not widely appreciated, prey can manage predation risk by modifying the sequence of their behavioural states. We explored this phenomenon in dugongs (Dugong dugon) subject to spatially and temporally variable risk of tiger shark (Galeocerdo cuvier) predation in Shark Bay, Australia. Dugong behaviour was assayed using focal follows and organised into sequences of foraging, resting, and travelling bouts. We used log-linear analysis to test for sequence differences in relation to habitat (deep, shallow) and predation danger (sharks present, largely absent). Dugongs modified their behavioural sequences between periods of high and low shark abundance: those at risk alternated more frequently between foraging, which constrains anti-predator vigilance, and travelling, which facilitates predator detection. Dugongs also avoided continuous series of resting bouts, during which awareness is reduced, when sharks were present. These changes were only observed in relatively dangerous shallow habitat, which is hunted disproportionately by tiger sharks; behavioural responses to sharks in deep habitat were modest. We conclude that dugongs in risky habitat resort to safer behavioural sequences in response to sharks. Given that human disturbance and predators are perceived similarly by many species, some forms of vessel interaction could compromise the fitness of sirenians by eliciting similar behavioural adjustment.

Additional keywords : Behavioral indicators, behavioral sequences, Dugong dugon, Galeocerdo cuvier, Markov chain, Shark Bay, tiger shark.


References

Altmann, J. (1974). Observational study of behavior: Sampling methods. Behaviour 49, 227–266.
Observational study of behavior: Sampling methods.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 1:STN:280:DyaE2c7mtlWmsQ%3D%3D&md5=b1d4f5b1de3e050759e368f3ef839af2CAS |

Anderson, P. K. (1995). Scarring and photoidentification of dugongs (Dugong dugon) in Shark Bay, Western Australia. Aquatic Mammals 21, 205–211.

Anderson, P. K., and Prince, R. I. T. (1985). Predation on dugongs: attacks by killer whales. Journal of Mammalogy 66, 554–556.

Anderson, D. R., Burnham, K. P., and Thompson, W. L. (2000). Null hypothesis testing: problems, prevalence, and an alternative. The Journal of Wildlife Management 64, 912–923.
Null hypothesis testing: problems, prevalence, and an alternative.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Brown, J. S., and Kotler, B. P. (2004). Hazardous duty pay and the foraging cost of predation. Ecology Letters 7, 999–1014.
Hazardous duty pay and the foraging cost of predation.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Burkholder, D. A., Fourqurean, J. W., and Heithaus, M. R. (In press). Spatial pattern in seagrass stoichiometry indicates both N-limited and P-limited regions of an iconic P-limited subtropical bay. Marine Ecology Progress Series. https://doi.org/10.3354/meps10042

Burnham, K. P., and Anderson, D. R. (1998). ‘Model Selection and Inference: A Practical Information-Theoretic Approach.’ (Springer-Verlag: New York.)

Burnham, K. P., Anderson, D. R., and Huyvaert, K. P. (2011). AIC model selection and multimodel inference in behavioral ecology: some background, observations, and comparisons. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 65, 23–35.
AIC model selection and multimodel inference in behavioral ecology: some background, observations, and comparisons.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Caswell, H. (2001). ‘Matrix Population Models.’ (Sinauer Associates: Boston.)

Chilvers, B. L., Delean, S., Gales, N. J., Holley, D. K., Lawler, I. R., Marsh, H., and Preen, A. R. (2004). Diving behaviour of dugongs, Dugong dugon. Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology 304, 203–224.
Diving behaviour of dugongs, Dugong dugon.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Cisneros-Montemayor, A. M., Sumalia, U. R., and Pauly, D. (2010). The global potential for whale watching. Marine Policy 34, 1273–1278.
The global potential for whale watching.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Desportes, J. P., Metcalfe, N. B., Popp, J. W., Meyer, R. M., Gallo, A., and Cézilly, F. (1990). The predictability and patterns of vigilant behaviour. Behavioural Processes 22, 41–46.
The predictability and patterns of vigilant behaviour.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Falcon-Matos, L., Mignucci-Giannoni, A. A., Toyos-Gonzalez, G. M., Bossart, G. D., Meisner, R. A., and Varela, R. A. (2003). Evidence of shark attack on a West Indian manatee (Trichechus manatus) in Puerto Rico. Journal of Neotropical Mammalogy 10, 161–166.

Ferriere, R., Cazelles, B., Cezilly, F., and Desportes, J. P. (1996). Predictability and chaos in bird vigilant behaviour. Animal Behaviour 52, 457–472.
Predictability and chaos in bird vigilant behaviour.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Fleiss, J. L. (1981). ‘Statistical Methods for Rates and Proportions.’ (Wiley: New York.)

Frid, A., and Dill, L. M. (2002). Human-caused disturbance stimuli as a form of predation risk. Conservation Ecology 6(1), 11. Available at http://www.consecol.org/vol6/iss1/art11/

Frid, A., Burns, J., Baker, G. G., and Thorne, R. E. (2009). Predicting synergistic effects of resources and predators on foraging decisions by juvenile Steller sea lions. Oecologia 158, 775–786.
Predicting synergistic effects of resources and predators on foraging decisions by juvenile Steller sea lions.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Gales, N., McCauley, R. D., Lanyon, J., and Holley, D. (2004). Change in abundance of dugongs in Shark Bay, Ningaloo and Exmouth Gulf, Western Australia: evidence for large-scale migration. Wildlife Research 31, 283–290.
Change in abundance of dugongs in Shark Bay, Ningaloo and Exmouth Gulf, Western Australia: evidence for large-scale migration.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Guttorp, P. (1995). ‘Stochastic Modelling of Scientific Data.’ (Chapman and Hall: New York.)

Heithaus, M. R. (2001). The biology of tiger sharks, Galeocerdo cuvier, in Shark Bay, Western Australia: sex ratio, size distribution, diet and seasonal changes in catch rates. Environmental Biology of Fishes 61, 25–36.
The biology of tiger sharks, Galeocerdo cuvier, in Shark Bay, Western Australia: sex ratio, size distribution, diet and seasonal changes in catch rates.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Heithaus, M. R., and Frid, A. (2003). Optimal diving under the risk of predation. Journal of Theoretical Biology 223, 79–92.
Optimal diving under the risk of predation.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Heithaus, M. R., Dill, L. M., Marshall, G. J., and Buhleier, B. M. (2002). Habitat use and foraging behavior of tiger sharks (Galeocerdo cuvier) in a subtropical seagrass ecosystem. Marine Biology 140, 229–236.

Heithaus, M. R., Hamilton, I. M., Wirsing, A. J., and Dill, L. M. (2006). Validation of a randomization procedure to assess animal habitat preferences: microhabitat use of tiger sharks in a seagrass ecosystem. Journal of Animal Ecology 75, 666–676.
Validation of a randomization procedure to assess animal habitat preferences: microhabitat use of tiger sharks in a seagrass ecosystem.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Heithaus, M. R., Wirsing, A. J., Frid, A., and Dill, L. M. (2007). Behavioral indicators in marine conservation: lessons from a pristine seagrass ecosystem. Israel Journal of Ecology and Evolution 53, 355–370.
Behavioral indicators in marine conservation: lessons from a pristine seagrass ecosystem.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Heithaus, M. R., Frid, A., Wirsing, A. J., and Worm, B. (2008). Predicting ecological consequences of marine top predator declines. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 23, 202–210.
Predicting ecological consequences of marine top predator declines.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Hodgson, A. J., and Marsh, H. (2007). Response of dugongs to boat traffic: the risk of disturbance and displacement. Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology 340, 50–61.
Response of dugongs to boat traffic: the risk of disturbance and displacement.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Jefferson, T. A., Stacey, P. J., and Baird, R. W. (1991). A review of killer whale interactions with other marine mammals: predation to co-existence. Mammal Review 21, 151–180.
A review of killer whale interactions with other marine mammals: predation to co-existence.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Lehner, P. N. (1987). Design and execution of animal behavior research: an overview. Journal of Animal Science 65, 1213–1219.
| 1:STN:280:DyaL1c%2Fos1GntQ%3D%3D&md5=1274ed1a76c8e5063920e470ddfe8c70CAS |

Lima, S. L. (1998). Stress and decision making under the risk of predation: recent developments from behavioral, reproductive, and ecological perspectives. Advances in the Study of Behavior 27, 215–290.
Stress and decision making under the risk of predation: recent developments from behavioral, reproductive, and ecological perspectives.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Lima, S. L., and Bednekoff, P. A. (1999). Back to the basics of antipredatory vigilance: can nonvigilant animals detect attack? Animal Behaviour 58, 537–543.
Back to the basics of antipredatory vigilance: can nonvigilant animals detect attack?Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Lima, S. L., and Dill, L. M. (1990). Behavioral decisions made under the risk of predation: a review and prospectus. Canadian Journal of Zoology 68, 619–640.
Behavioral decisions made under the risk of predation: a review and prospectus.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Lima, S. L., Rattenborg, N. C., Lesku, J. A., and Amlaner, C. J. (2005). Sleeping under the risk of predation. Animal Behaviour 70, 723–736.
Sleeping under the risk of predation.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Lusseau, D. (2003). Effects of tour boats on the behavior of bottlenose dolphins: Using Markov chains to model anthropogenic impacts. Conservation Biology 17, 1785–1793.
Effects of tour boats on the behavior of bottlenose dolphins: Using Markov chains to model anthropogenic impacts.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Lusseau, D. (2004). The hidden cost of tourism: detecting long-term effects of tourism using behavioral information. Ecology and Society 9(1): 2. Available at http://www.ecologyandsociety.org/vol9/iss1/art2

Lusseau, D., Bain, D. E., Williams, R., and Smith, J. C. (2009). Vessel traffic disrupts the foraging behavior of southern resident killer whales Orcinus orca. Endangered Species Research 6, 211–221.
Vessel traffic disrupts the foraging behavior of southern resident killer whales Orcinus orca.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Marsh, H., Channells, P. W., Heinsohn, G. E., and Morrissey, J. (1982). Analysis of stomach contents of dugongs from Queensland. Wildlife Research 9, 55–67.
Analysis of stomach contents of dugongs from Queensland.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Masini, R. J., Anderson, P. K., and McComb, A. J. (2001). A Halodule-dominated community in a subtropical embayment: physical environment, productivity, biomass, and impact of dugong grazing. Aquatic Botany 71, 179–197.
A Halodule-dominated community in a subtropical embayment: physical environment, productivity, biomass, and impact of dugong grazing.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Matich, P., Heithaus, M. R., and Layman, C. A. (2011). Contrasting patterns of individual specialization and trophic coupling in two marine apex predators. Journal of Animal Ecology 80, 294–305.
Contrasting patterns of individual specialization and trophic coupling in two marine apex predators.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

McCarthy, E. (2004). ‘International Regulation of Underwater Sound: Establishing Rules and Standards to Address Ocean Noise Pollution.’ (Kluwer Academic Publishers: Boston, MA.)

Miksis-Olds, J. L., Donaghay, P. L., Miller, J. H., Tyack, P. L., and Reynolds, J. E. (2007). Simulated vessel approaches elicit differential responses from manatees. Marine Mammal Science 23, 629–649.
Simulated vessel approaches elicit differential responses from manatees.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Nowacek, S. M., Wells, R. S., Owen, E. C. G., Speakman, T. R., Flamm, R. O., and Nowacek, D. P. (2004). Florida manatees, Trichechus manatus latirostris, respond to approaching vessels. Biological Conservation 119, 517–523.
Florida manatees, Trichechus manatus latirostris, respond to approaching vessels.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Parrish, F. A., Craig, M. P., Ragen, T. J., Marshall, G. J., and Buhleier, B. M. (2000). Identifying diurnal foraging habitat of endangered Hawaiian monk seals using a seal-mounted video camera. Marine Mammal Science 16, 392–412.
Identifying diurnal foraging habitat of endangered Hawaiian monk seals using a seal-mounted video camera.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Sargeant, B. L., and Mann, J. (2009). Developmental evidence for foraging traditions in wild bottlenose dolphins. Animal Behaviour 78, 715–721.
Developmental evidence for foraging traditions in wild bottlenose dolphins.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Simpfendorfer, C. A., Goodreid, A. B., and McAuley, R. B. (2001). Size, sex and geographic variation in the diet of the tiger shark, Galeocerdo cuvier, from Western Australian waters. Environmental Biology of Fishes 61, 37–46.
Size, sex and geographic variation in the diet of the tiger shark, Galeocerdo cuvier, from Western Australian waters.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Walker, D. I., Kendrick, G. A., and McComb, A. J. (1988). The distribution of seagrass species in Shark Bay, Western Australia, with notes on their ecology. Aquatic Botany 30, 305–317.
The distribution of seagrass species in Shark Bay, Western Australia, with notes on their ecology.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Wirsing, A. J., Heithaus, M. R., and Dill, L. M. (2006). Tiger shark (Galeocerdo cuvier) abundance and growth in a subtropical embayment: evidence from 7 years of standardized fishing effort. Marine Biology 149, 961–968.
Tiger shark (Galeocerdo cuvier) abundance and growth in a subtropical embayment: evidence from 7 years of standardized fishing effort.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Wirsing, A. J., Heithaus, M. R., and Dill, L. M. (2007a). Fear factor: do dugongs (Dugong dugon) trade food for safety from tiger sharks (Galeocerdo cuvier)? Oecologia 153, 1031–1040.
Fear factor: do dugongs (Dugong dugon) trade food for safety from tiger sharks (Galeocerdo cuvier)?Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Wirsing, A. J., Heithaus, M. R., and Dill, L. M. (2007b). Can measures of prey availability improve our ability to predict the abundance of large marine predators? Oecologia 153, 563–568.
Can measures of prey availability improve our ability to predict the abundance of large marine predators?Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Wirsing, A. J., Heithaus, M. R., and Dill, L. M. (2007c). Living on the edge: dugongs prefer to forage in microhabitats offering escape from rather than avoidance of predators. Animal Behaviour 74, 93–101.
Living on the edge: dugongs prefer to forage in microhabitats offering escape from rather than avoidance of predators.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Wirsing, A. J., Heithaus, M. R., and Dill, L. M. (2007d). Can you dig it? Use of excavation, a risky foraging tactic, is sensitive to predation danger. Animal Behaviour 74, 1085–1091.
Can you dig it? Use of excavation, a risky foraging tactic, is sensitive to predation danger.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Wirsing, A. J., Heithaus, M. R., Frid, A., and Dill, L. M. (2008). Seascapes of fear: evaluating sublethal predator effects experienced and generated by marine mammals. Marine Mammal Science 24, 1–15.
Seascapes of fear: evaluating sublethal predator effects experienced and generated by marine mammals.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Worthington, A. M., and Swallow, J. G. (2011). Sequential analysis reveals behavioral differences underlying female-biased predation risk in stalk-eyed flies. Ethology 117, 829–837.
Sequential analysis reveals behavioral differences underlying female-biased predation risk in stalk-eyed flies.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |