Training Merino sheep to respond to visual and auditory cues
Donnalee B. Taylor A C D , Wendy Y. Brown A , Ian R. Price B and Geoff N. Hinch A CA School of Environmental and Rural Sciences – Animal Science, University of New England, Armidale, NSW 2351, Australia.
B School of Cognitive, Behavioural and Social Sciences – Psychology, University of New England, Armidale, NSW 2351, Australia.
C Australian Sheep Industry Cooperative Research Centre, Armidale, NSW 2351, Australia.
D Corresponding author. Email: dtaylor2@une.edu.au
Animal Production Science 50(6) 541-545 https://doi.org/10.1071/AN10003
Submitted: 8 January 2010 Accepted: 2 May 2010 Published: 11 June 2010
Abstract
The feasibility of training sheep to approach a stimulus was investigated in three experimental groups and a control group of fine wool Merino sheep (n = 11 in each group). The experimental groups (n = 11) were trained to approach either a visual (V), auditory (A), or visual + auditory (V+A) stimulus over eight training sessions and were subsequently tested in a T-maze for memory retention on six occasions over a 4-month period. Four testing occasions were spaced at greater than 30 days apart while two were less than 3 days apart. Sheep learned to approach the cues during the training period and the tests indicated that the sheep retained memory of the cues for over 130 days without reinforcement. The controls received no contingency exposure and made no choice in the T-maze test. The proportion of correct stimulus choices (±s.e.m.) in the T-maze averaged over the four longer-spaced testing occasions were V 0.61 (0.06), A 0.50 (0.11), V+A 0.77 (0.04). These differences approached significance (P = 0.058). Sheep trained to the V+A stimulus performed significantly (P < 0.05) better in the T-maze than sheep trained to the A stimulus alone. Comparisons over the shorter-spaced testing durations indicated that the sheep quickly learnt not to approach the stimulus (temporary extinction) when no food reward was available. Individual temperament of the animals was not related to their learning. This study highlights the potential for the use of V and A cues in manipulating the movement of sheep which may be useful for farm management purposes.
Acknowledgements
The University of New England (UNE), Australian Wool Education Trust Fund and Sheep CRC generously funded this research. The UNE Science and Engineering Workshop team’s ingenious construction of trial equipment was greatly appreciated. The authors also extend their appreciation to Paul A. Arnott, Neil D. Baillie, Graham A. Chaffey, Mark D. Porter, Michael G. Raue and Gary W. Taylor, for their assistance in trial setup and animal care.
Alexander G,
Lynch JJ, Mottershead BE
(1979) Use of shelter and selection of lambing sites by shorn and unshorn ewes in paddocks with closely or widely spaced shelters. Applied Animal Ethology 5, 51–69.
| Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
Bailey DW
(1996) Mechanisms that result in large herbivore grazing distribution patterns. Journal of Range Management 49(5), 386–400.
| Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
Bailey DW,
Rittenhouse LR,
Hart RH,
Swift DM, Richards RW
(1989b) Association of relative food availabilities and locations by cattle. Journal of Range Management 42(6), 480–482.
| Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
Baldwin BA
(1972) Operant conditioning techniques for the study of thermo-regulatory behaviour in sheep. The Journal of Physiology 226, 41–42.
Behrends SM,
Miller RK,
Rouquette FM,
Randel RD, Warrington BG ,
et al
.
(2009) Relationship of temperament, growth, carcass characteristics and tenderness in beef steers. Meat Science 81(3), 433–438.
| Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
Benhamou S
(1994) Spatial memory and searching efficiency. Animal Behaviour 47, 1423–1433.
| Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
Bremner KJ,
Braggins JB, Kilgour R
(1980) Training sheep as ‘leaders’ in abattoirs and farm sheep yards. Proceedings of the New Zealand Society of Animal Production 40, 111–116.
Burrow HM
(1997) Measurements of temperament and their relationships with performance traits of beef cattle. Animal Breeding Abstract 65(7), 477–495.
Dumont B, Petit M
(1998) Spatial memory of sheep at pasture. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 60, 43–53.
| Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
Galef BG, Laland KN
(2005) Social learning in animals: empirical studies and theoretical models. BioSciences 55, 489–499.
| Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
Lee C,
Colegate S, Fisher AD
(2006) Development of a maze test and its application to assess spatial learning and memory in Merino sheep. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 96, 43–51.
| Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
Mellor DJ
(2004) Taming and training of pregnant sheep and goats and of newborn lambs, kids, and calves before experimentation. ATLA 32(Suppl. 1), 143–146.
|
CAS |
Rescorla RA
(2004) Spontaneous recovery. Cold Spring Harbour Laboratory Press 11, 501–509.