Vocal behaviour of the squirrel glider (Petaurus norfolcensis)
David J. Sharpe A B and Ross L. Goldingay AA School of Environmental Science and Management, Southern Cross University, PO Box 157, Lismore, NSW 2480, Australia.
B Corresponding author. Email: davidsharpe3@bigpond.com
Australian Journal of Zoology 57(1) 55-64 https://doi.org/10.1071/ZO08075
Submitted: 16 September 2008 Accepted: 19 April 2009 Published: 29 May 2009
Abstract
We describe the vocal behaviour of the squirrel glider (Petaurus norfolcensis) from 465 h of observation across five sites in north-east New South Wales and south-east Queensland. A monosyllabic or polysyllabic nasal grunt was the most frequent call (56% of 208 calls); it ranged from single calls to sequences of up to 20-min duration (mean 2.1 min ± 0.6, s.e.) and was heard on 34% of nights (n = 83) at two sites. The rate of the nasal grunt showed a positive relationship with population density at one site. The nasal grunt was typically made when conspecifics were near the caller, but responses were infrequent (7% of observations). Call playback produced no discernable change in call response. The nasal grunt appears to regulate individual spacing by facilitating mutual avoidance, a function hypothesised to be an evolutionary precursor to the use of calls in territorial defence. Threatening calls were the next most common vocalisation (17% of calls) and were accompanied by scuffles and/or chases. They were also used when gliders were preyed upon and during animal handling. The calling behaviour of the squirrel glider confirms the importance of vocal communication among petaurid gliders.
Acknowledgements
We thank Craig Taylor for on-going technical support of our field studies and for conducting call playback at Larnook. Brendan Taylor is thanked for the use of his unpublished observations from Karawatha Forest Reserve. Brisbane City Council, the NSW NPWS and NSW State Forests are thanked for allowing access to their estate. Brisbane City Council is thanked for providing financial support of our research in Brisbane.
Beyer, G. L. , Goldingay, R. L. , and Sharpe, D. J. (2008). The characteristics of squirrel glider (Petaurus norfolcensis) den trees in subtropical Australia. Australian Journal of Zoology 56, 13–21.
| Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
Blumstein, D. T. , and Armitage, K. B. (1997). Does sociality drive the evolution of communicative complexity? A comparative test with ground-dwelling sciurid alarm calls. American Naturalist 150, 179–200.
| Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | CAS | PubMed |
Dobson, M. , Goldingay, R. , and Sharpe, D. (2005). Feeding behaviour of the squirrel glider in remnant habitat in Brisbane. Australian Mammalogy 27, 27–35.
Goldingay, R. L. , and Sharpe, D. J. (2004). How effective is spotlighting for detecting the squirrel glider? Wildlife Research 31, 443–449.
| Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
Holland, G. , Bennett, A. F. , and van der Ree, R. (2007). Time-budget and feeding behaviour of the squirrel glider (Petaurus norfolcensis) in remnant linear habitat. Wildlife Research 34, 288–295.
| Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
Lair, H. (1990). The calls of the red squirrel: a contextual analysis of function. Behaviour 115, 254–281.
| Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
Menkhorst, P. W. , and Collier, M. (1987). Diet of the squirrel glider, Petaurus norfolcensis (Marsupialia: Petauridae), in Victoria. Australian Mammalogy 11, 109–116.
Sabatini, V. , and Ruiz-Miranda, C. R. (2008). Acoustical aspects of the propagation of long calls of wild Leontopithecus rosalia. International Journal of Primatology 29, 207–223.
| Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
Sekulic, R. (1982). The function of howling in red howler monkeys (Allouatta seniculus). Behaviour 81, 38–54.
| Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
Sharpe, D. J. , and Goldingay, R. L. (1998). Feeding behaviour of the squirrel glider at Bungawalbin Nature Reserve, north-eastern New South Wales. Wildlife Research 25, 243–254.
| Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
Stoddart, D. M. , and Bradley, A. J. (1991). The frontal and gular dermal scent organs of the marsupial sugar glider (Petaurus breviceps). Journal of Zoology 225, 1–12.
| Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
Tang, Y. Z. , Zhuang, L. Z. , and Wang, Z. W. (2001). Advertisement calls and their relation to reproductive cycles in Gekko gecko (Reptilia, Lacertilia). Copeia 2001, 248–253.
| Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
Traill, B. J. (1998). Identification of squirrel gliders and sugar gliders in Victoria. The Victorian Naturalist 115, 135–141.
Traill, B. J. , and Lill, A. (1997). Use of tree hollows by two sympatric gliding possums, the squirrel glider, Petaurus norfolcensis and the sugar glider, P. breviceps. Australian Mammalogy 20, 79–88.
van der Ree, R. (2002). The population ecology of the squirrel glider, Petaurus norfolcensis, within a network of remnant linear habitats. Wildlife Research 29, 329–340.
| Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
van der Ree, R. , and Bennett, A. F. (2003). Home range of the squirrel glider (Petaurus norfolcensis) in a network of remnant linear habitats. Journal of Zoology 259, 327–336.
| Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
Van Dyck, S. (1993). The taxonomy and distribution of Petaurus gracilis (Marsupialia: Petauridae), with notes on its ecology and conservation status. Memoirs of the Queensland Museum 33, 77–122.
Waser, P. M. (1975). Experimental playbacks show vocal mediation of intergroup avoidance in a forest monkey. Nature 255, 56–58.
| Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
Waser, P. M. (1977). Individual recognition, intragroup cohesion and intergroup spacing: evidence from sound playback to forest monkeys. Behaviour 60, 28–74.
| Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
Whitehead, J. M. (1987). Vocally mediated reciprocity between neighbouring groups of mantled howling monkeys, Alouatta palliata palliata. Animal Behaviour 35, 1615–1627.
| Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
Wich, S. A. , and Nunn, C. L. (2002). Do male “long-distance calls” function in mate defense? A comparative study of long-distance calls in primates. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 52, 474–484.
| Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
Wong, B. B. , Cowling, A. N. N. , Cunningham, R. B. , Donnelly, C. F. , and Cooper, P. D. (2004). Do temperature and social environment interact to affect call rate in frogs (Crinia signifera)? Austral Ecology 29, 209–214.
| Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |