The temporal organisation of the feeding behaviour of four species of wallaby: examining chronobiological and homeostatic influences
R. G. Lentle A E , I. D. Hume B , K. J. Stafford C , M. Kennedy B , B. P. Springett A and S. Haslett DA Institute of Natural Resources Ecology, College of Sciences, Massey University, Private Bag 11222, Palmerston North, New Zealand.
B School of Biological Sciences A08, University of Sydney, NSW 2006, Australia.
C Institute of Veterinary, Animal and Biomedical Sciences, College of Sciences, Massey University, Private Bag 11222, Palmerston North, New Zealand.
D Statistics Research and Consultancy Centre, Massey University, Private Bag 11222, Palmerston North, New Zealand.
E Corresponding author. Email: r.g.lentle@massey.ac.nz
Australian Journal of Zoology 53(2) 117-129 https://doi.org/10.1071/ZO03006
Submitted: 28 January 2003 Accepted: 7 February 2005 Published: 6 April 2005
Abstract
We studied the temporal distribution of feeding events of tammar wallabies, parma wallabies, brush-tailed rock-wallabies and swamp wallabies by infrared video-recording of captive animals maintained in a 12 : 12 light : dark cycle with constant temperature, and by radio-microphone surveillance of free-ranging animals on Kawau Island, New Zealand. Analyses of mean time spent feeding per 20-min period for tammar and parma wallabies showed significant circadian variation in both captive and free-ranging animals but not in captive or free-ranging brush-tailed rock wallabies or swamp wallabies. While there was no significant difference in the extent of circadian variation of feeding rate between tammar and parma wallabies there were significant differences according to the type of food being fed. Thus, circadian variation in feeding rate was greater in both species when standing sward was fed. None of the survivorship curves of the interfeed intervals of either captive or free-ranging animals showed a discontinuity suggestive of organisation of feed events into meals. There were bicrepuscular peaks of feed-event density in both captive and free-ranging tammar and parma wallabies. There was a similar bicrepuscular distribution of feed-event density in free-ranging brush-tailed rock-wallabies but a more uniform temporal distribution of this parameter in captive specimens. There was a more uniform circadian distribution of feed-event density in a single captive and a single free-ranging swamp wallaby. Thus the circadian incidence of the feeding events of tammar and parma wallabies were more resistant to changes in the incidence of stochastic environmental variables such as predation whereas those of bush-tailed rock-wallabies and swamp wallabies were more susceptible to changes in these stimuli.
Acknowledgments
We thank the Department of Conservation, Kawau Island, and staff and students of the Department of Ecology, Massey University, for assistance with field work. This work was completed with ARC grant fund to I. D. Hume and B. P. S. Springett. The research was conducted with approval of the Massey University Ethics Committee (Approval Number 98/186).
Blundell, J. E. , and Halford, J. C. G. (1994). Regulation of nutrient supply; the brain and appetite control. Proceedings of the Nutrition Society 53, 407–418.
| PubMed |
Caughley, G. (1964). Social organisation and daily activity of the red kangaroo and the grey kangaroo. Journal of Mammalogy 45, 429–436.
Croft, D. B. (1981). Behaviour of red kangaroos Macropus rufus (Desmarest 1822) in northwest New South Wales, Australia Australian Mammalogy 4, 5–58.
de Haer, L. C. M. , and de Vries, A. G. (1993). Feed intake patterns of and feed digestibility in growing pigs housed individually or in groups. Livestock Production Science 33, 277–292.
| Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
Demment, M. W. , and Van Soest, P. J. (1985). A nutritional explanation for body-size patterns of ruminant and non-ruminant herbivores. American Naturalist 125, 641–672.
| Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
Grant, T. R. (1974). Observations of enclosed and free-ranging grey kangaroos (Macropus giganteus). Zeitschrift fur Saugetierekunde 39, 65–78.
Johnson, D. F. , and Collier, G. H. (1987). Caloric regulation and patterns of food choice in a patchy environment: the value and cost of alternative foods. Physiology & Behavior 39, 351–359.
| Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | PubMed |
Lentle, R. G. , Potter, M. , Stafford, K. J. , Springett, B. P. , and Haslett, S. (1997). A trapping and immobilisation technique for small macropods. Wildlife Research 24, 373–377.
| Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
Murphy, P. , and Campbell, S. S. (1996). Physiology of circadian rhythms in animals and humans. Journal of Clinical Neurophysiology 13, 2–16.
| Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | PubMed |
Schoener, T. W. (1983). Simple models of optimal foraging territory size: a reconciliation. American Naturalist 121, 608–669.
| Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
Slater, P. J. B. , and Lester, N. P. (1982). Minimising errors in splitting behaviour into bouts. Behaviour 79, 153–161.
Staddon, J. E. , Chelaru, I. M. , and Higa, J. J. (2002). Habituation, memory and the brain: the dynamics of interval timing. Behavioural Processes 57, 71–88.
| Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | PubMed |
Waterhouse, J. , Minors, D. , Atkinson, G. , and Benton, D. (1997). Chronobiology and meal times: internal and external factors. British Journal of Nutrition 77(Suppl. 1), S29–S38.
| PubMed |
Wodzicki, K. , and Flux, J. E. C. (1967). Guide to introduced wallabies in New Zealand. Tuatara 15, 47–59.