Differential space use inferred from live trapping versus telemetry: northern flying squirrels and fine spatial grain
Matthew Wheatley A C D and Karl Larsen A BA Biology Department, University of Victoria, Box 3020 STN CSC, Victoria, British Columbia V8W 3N5, Canada.
B Department of Natural Resource Sciences, Thompson Rivers University, Box 3010, Kamloops, British Columbia V2C 5N3, Canada.
C Present address: Alberta Parks and Protected Areas, 3rd Floor, Government Center, 131 Civic Center Road, Hinton, Alberta T7V 2E6, Canada.
D Corresponding author. Email: matthew.wheatley@gov.ab.ca
Wildlife Research 35(5) 425-433 https://doi.org/10.1071/WR07082
Submitted: 3 July 2007 Accepted: 21 April 2008 Published: 19 August 2008
Abstract
Small mammal space use is inferred from live-capture data or various methods of tracking, with differences between these methods potentially affecting the input and subsequent inferential abilities of resulting wildlife-habitat models. Unlike tracking via radio telemetry, live trapping employs use of bait, which is known to change proximate animal density as evident in many food addition studies (the ‘pantry effect’), and conceivably bias individuals’ space use, particularly if measured over small spatial extents in heterogeneous areas. The present study analysed both trapping and telemetry data from northern flying squirrels (Glaucomys sabrinus) to assess whether different habitat associations could be generated based on methods alone. Conditional on sampling method, two different space-use patterns were identified from the same group of squirrels and two significantly different sets of habitat model input were associated with each. Trap areas were not used post capture; once enumerated, animals on average (n = 34) spent over 80% of their time from 100 to 200+ m, upwards of 800 m, away from trap areas. Using telemetry and fine-grained habitat structure data, this study found 33% of sampled squirrels used areas not identified via habitat-stratified trap effort (specifically black spruce habitat). It is concluded that wildlife-habitat investigations dealing with fine spatial grain are likely to acquire different results using trapping versus telemetry, especially if animals are relatively mobile and habitat structure is relatively heterogeneous.
Additional keywords: extent, Glaucomys sabrinus, habitat modelling, heterogeneity, grain, live trapping, radio telemetry, scale, transect sampling.
Acknowledgements
For assistance in the field, we thank C. Bird, T. de Monye, A. McCaffrey and B. Purvis. This research was supported via a scholarship to MW and a grant to KL from the Natural Science and Engineering Research Council of Canada, grants to MW from the Canadian Wildlife Foundation, the Alberta Sport, Parks, Recreation and Wildlife Foundation, the University of Alberta Challenge Grants in Biodiversity, the Forest Resource Improvement Association of Alberta, and funds from Hinton Wood Products Ltd (a division of West Fraser Mills Ltd). We thank two anonymous reviewers for helpful comments on an earlier draft of this manuscript.
Ball, S. J. , Lindenmayer, D. B. , and Possingham, H. P. (2003). The predictive accuracy of population viability analysis: a test using data from two small mammal species in a fragmented landscape. Biodiversity and Conservation 12, 2393–2413.
| Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
Gustafson, E. J. (1998). Minireview: quantifying landscape spatial pattern: what is the state of the art? Ecosystems (New York, N.Y.) 1, 143–156.
| Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
Levin, S. A. (1992). The problem of pattern and scale in ecology. Ecology 73, 1943–1967.
| Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
Menzel, J. M. , Ford, W. M. , Edwards, J. W. , and Terry, T. M. (2006). Home range and habitat use of the Vulnerable Virginia northern flying squirrel Glaucomys sabrinus fuscus in the Central Appalachian Mountains, USA. Oryx 40, 204–210.
| Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
Zimmerman, G. S. , and Glanz, W. E. (2000). Habitat use by bats in eastern Maine. The Journal of Wildlife Management 64, 1032–1040.
| Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |