Free Standard AU & NZ Shipping For All Book Orders Over $80!
Register      Login
Wildlife Research Wildlife Research Society
Ecology, management and conservation in natural and modified habitats
RESEARCH ARTICLE (Open Access)

Reducing koala roadkill: a social marketing formative study

Bo Pang https://orcid.org/0000-0003-3227-3612 A * , Anran Zhang B , Tori Seydel A , Patricia David https://orcid.org/0000-0001-6655-2500 A , Murooj Yousef A and Sharyn Rundle-Thiele A
+ Author Affiliations
- Author Affiliations

A Griffith University, Griffith Business School, Social Marketing @ Griffith, 170 Kessels Road, Nathan, Qld, Australia.

B School of Economics & Management, Xidian University, Xi’an, China.

* Correspondence to: b.pang@griffith.edu.au

Handling Editor: Peter Brown

Wildlife Research 50(10) 858-868 https://doi.org/10.1071/WR21172
Submitted: 28 August 2020  Accepted: 2 December 2022   Published: 30 January 2023

© 2023 The Author(s) (or their employer(s)). Published by CSIRO Publishing. This is an open access article distributed under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License (CC BY-NC-ND)

Abstract

Context: Koalas are an Australian icon and their numbers are seriously declining. In some regions, a key threat to koalas is vehicle strikes. Therefore, understanding what might convince people to be more vigilant and willing to slow down in areas with high koala numbers is an important research undertaking.

Aims: This study aims to use the wildlife value orientation (WVO) framework, a value-based segmentation process, to extend conservation insight. It will do this by demonstrating the application of social cognitive theory (SCT), to investigate how wildlife beliefs can help in identifying worthwhile groups to target with wildlife conservation interventions. The findings of this study can be used to benefit koalas by assisting conservation planning efforts to decrease driving speed and promote positive changes in driving behaviour.

Method: Data collection through intercept surveys was employed by convenience sampling in 2019. A total of 661 responses were collected from residents of a koala priority conservation area in Queensland, Australia. Respondents were asked about their wildlife values, beliefs, attitudes, norms, barriers, and intentions to slow down, while driving in a koala area. Four key groups were identified based on respondents’ WVO (i.e. mutualists, pluralists, traditionalists and distanced) and multigroup structural equation modelling was conducted to understand group differences.

Key results: The analysis identified several significant psychographic factors that influenced people’s intentions to protect koalas dependent on what wildlife value respondents held. Injunctive norms were important, positively influencing the intention of traditionalists and pluralists to slow down while driving in a koala area. Individual attitude positively influenced safe driving intention for pluralists, while perceived barriers negatively influenced mutualists’ intention to slow down. Other groups did not show similar results.

Conclusion: A person’s WVO can influence their intention to protect koalas by modifying their driving behaviour. Various social marketing approaches can benefit conservation strategies aimed at different WVO groups with targeted messages and interventions for each group.

Implications: This paper demonstrates the value of SCT in explaining people’s intention to slow down to protect koalas. The identification of group differences demonstrates that varied approaches are required to deliver behavioural change to benefit koalas.

Keywords: behaviour, behavioural change, human dimensions, koala, segmentation, social marketing, theoretical modelling, wildlife.

Introduction

Koalas are an Australian icon and their numbers are seriously declining in the northern part of their range, specifically New South Wales (NSW) and Queensland (Gonzalez-Astudillo 2019; Lunney et al. 2022) Recent bushfires have halved populations in some regions (Trask 2020). Human factors such as increased urbanisation create further pressure, where vehicle strikes are a major threat to koalas (Shumway et al. 2015; Queensland Department of Environment and Science 2019; Department of Agriculture Water and the Environment 2022; Lunney et al. 2022). A recent parliamentary enquiry in NSW (NSW Parliament Enquiry 2020) found that ‘a key threat to local koala populations were roads, traffic and vehicle strikes.’ While natural disasters such as bushfires highly impact koala population (with 6382 koalas killed in the 2019/2020 bushfire season in NSW alone; (International Fund for Animal Welfare 2022), other factors are also considered key drivers of koala mortality (Gonzalez-Astudillo 2019). Such factors include urbanisation of coastal areas (McAlpine et al. 2015), domestic dog attacks (David et al. 2019) and vehicle strikes (Lunney et al. 2022). The current study focuses on koala roadkill, and strategies to positively influence safe driving behaviour in highly populated koala regions. Every year around 300 koalas are killed on South East Queensland roads, while NSW reported 3500 koalas killed by vehicles between 1980 and 2018 on its roads (Department of Environment and Science 2020; Department of Planning, Industry and Environment 2020). Local councils mitigate risks to koalas with measures including koala-safe passages, static slow-down signs and koala injury reporting systems (Redland City Council 2016). Given that human factors also contribute to koala mortality, the provision of infrastructure cannot be solely relied upon. This study responds to the call for a better understanding of human dimensions in the discipline of conservation, and factors that may enable people to be more vigilant and willing to slow down in areas with high koala numbers.

Human dimensions should be considered for biodiversity conservation to be implemented successfully (Robinson et al. 2019; Veríssimo and Wan 2019). While the importance of human dimensions in improving conservation has been previously discussed, the literature demonstrates a lack of understanding of how human dimensions can be employed for positive behaviour change outcomes (Bennett et al. 2017). This indicates a need for empirical research to extend understanding of the role of human dimensions in conservation interventions and its effect on conservation management outcomes. A field known for its focus on human behaviour change is social marketing. Social marketing has been applied to a broad range of social issues such as increasing physical activity (Kubacki et al. 2017a), minimising alcohol harm (Yousef et al. 2021) and encouraging positive wildlife conservation behaviour (Rundle-Thiele et al. 2019a). Use of social marketing techniques to ensure effective behaviour change interventions to benefit wildlife conservation has received increasing attention (Robinson et al. 2019). Examples of the successful application of social marketing to enhance wildlife conservation are emerging (David et al. 2019; Rundle-Thiele et al. 2019a). Common across literature reviews (Truong 2014; Kubacki et al. 2017b) is a lack of theory and segmentation use, despite the known benefits associated with the application of these two key social marketing principles (Rundle-Thiele et al. 2019b).

Heterogeneity in the human population is reflected in differences in psychographic factors proven to influence human behaviour (Dahana et al. 2018). Hence, segmentation (i.e. classification of people into groups based on shared characteristics) is increasingly being used by researchers to help enable a more targeted behaviour change approach to groups with different psychographic factors such as beliefs, attitudes and norms (Jones et al. 2019). McKenzie-Mohr et al. (2012) highlight that the foundation of environmental protection is human behavioural change. Understanding why people do, or do not, perform behaviours remains a key priority to ensure solutions devised to address conservation issues take human dimensions into account.

This enquiry is underpinned by two frameworks. The first, social cognitive theory (SCT) (Bandura 1989), considers behaviour as a triadic reciprocal process with personal, social/environmental factors and behavioural factors continually interacting to affect overall behaviour (Glanz 2001) The second framework is wildlife value orientation (WVO) (Teel and Manfredo 2010).

According to the cognitive hierarchy model of human behaviour (Homer and Kahle 1988), WVO is the set of beliefs relating to wildlife and human coexistence represented as clusters of interrelated fundamental values (Fulton et al. 1996). The WVO framework has been used to segment a target audience based on how they value wildlife. The framework captures the nuances of people’s beliefs and their relationships with wildlife. These values are important as they have been proven to have a direct effect on people’s daily behaviour in terms of interaction with wildlife (Teel and Manfredo 2010). WVO consists of two value orientations (domination and mutualism) and four belief dimensions: hunting and use of wildlife, and caring and social affiliation (Manfredo et al. 2009; Teel and Manfredo 2010). A domination WVO reflects the belief that wildlife should be managed for human benefit. Individuals with this orientation are more likely to prioritise human well-being over wildlife. A mutualism WVO reflects an egalitarian ideology that has fostered perceptions of social inclusion and equality that extend to human–animal relationships (Steel et al. 1994). Individuals with a mutualism orientation view wildlife as capable of relationships of trust with humans, as if wildlife were part of an extended family, and thus deserving of rights and care. These individuals are more likely to engage in welfare-enhancing behaviours for wildlife. To categorise individuals based on their WVO, Teel and Manfredo (2010) suggested individuals could be divided into four types: traditionalist (high domination–low mutualism), mutualist (low domination–high mutualism), pluralist (high domination–high mutualism) and distanced (low domination–low mutualism). These groups differ in wildlife-related attitudes and behaviours, and the WVO may offer a foundational segmentation identification tool that can be applied to manage human–wildlife conflict issues more effectively.

Segmentation, a tool used in commercial and social marketing, can deliver a nuanced understanding of human dimensions, demonstrating differences that can inform policy selection, strategy development, program delivery, and program evaluation (French 2017). Detailed segmentation modelling ensures that interventions cater to the needs of various groups exhibiting clear differences in beliefs and behaviour, allowing for more refined program delivery and evaluation (French 2017). Segmentation has been gaining popularity in the discipline of conservation research (Hinsley et al. 2015; Jones et al. 2019; Metcalf et al. 2019) as ecologists and conservationists realise the value of applying processes that can assist them to consider the diversity of human behaviour. For example, a recent study by Mehta and Chahal (2021) applies segmentation techniques to understand consumer groups based on their environmental attitudes; the identified segments can then be targeted and engaged differently by social marketers for more effective outcomes. Our study extends conservation insight by demonstrating the application of the WVO framework alongside a behavioural change theory to investigate how wildlife beliefs can form key segments, enabling a targeted behaviour change approach to benefit wildlife (e.g. safe driving).

Specifically, this study applies four social marketing principles: consumer orientation, use of behavioural theory, insights and segmentation (French and Russell-Bennett 2015). The study hence employs a consumer-oriented and theoretically guided empirical enquiry, delivering segmented insights to inform the design of campaigns to reduce koala roadkill.


Literature review and hypothesis formation

A focus of SCT is self-regulation, in which individuals drive their self-conceptions, revise behaviour, adjust their environment, and act to bring about outcomes that match with their own goals and self-perceptions (Plotnikoff et al. 2013; Sawitri et al. 2015). The SCT has been employed and validated in contexts including pro-environmental behaviours (Tabernero and Hernández 2011; Tang et al. 2011) and conservation (Sawitri et al. 2015). SCT focuses on the interaction of personal, environmental, and behavioural factors. Personal factors include attitude and outcome expectations (Bandura 1989). Environmental factors focus on the influence of others and/or the environment (Bandura 1989) and are considered by many as social and situational. SCT considers the relationships between these constructs as reciprocal and dynamic. To date, there has been minimal empirical application of SCT to ascertain the extent to which it offers explanatory potential that can be reliably used to inform program and campaign planning as well as intervention design (Rakotonirainy et al. 2014; Chin et al. 2017). SCT was applied in this study to identify determinants of behaviour.

In driving and road safety contexts, the application of SCT is limited and focuses predominantly on social norms as a method to influence driving behaviour (Chin et al. 2017). In this study, four constructs of the SCT (outcome expectation, attitudes, social norms and perceived barriers) were used to explain the context.

Outcome expectation is a social cognitive variable that refers to the anticipatory (positive or negative) consequences of engaging in a behaviour (Hatchett et al. 2013; Lent et al. 2017). The second construct within the SCT is attitudes, referring to an individual’s consistent tendency to respond favourably or unfavourably toward an object (Vaske and Donnelly 1999). Generally, behaviour follows a reasoned action approach, which assumes behaviour is a result of one’s perceptions including beliefs, attitudes and social norms (Ajzen and Fishbein 2005). Research suggests that an individual’s intention to perform a behaviour is based on having a positive attitude towards it (Hrubes et al. 2001). Hence, we hypothesise:

H1: Outcome expectation has a positive effect on the intention to slow down when driving in a koala populated area.

H2: A favourable attitude towards wildlife has a positive effect on the intention to slow down when driving in a koala populated area.

Social norms refer to a person’s perceptions of social pressure placed upon them to perform (or not) a particular behaviour (Beedell and Rehman 2000; Wauters et al. 2010). Injunctive social norms reflect ‘what is approved or disapproved by society’, while descriptive social norms capture ‘how most important others behave in a given situation’ (Reid and Aiken 2013). We hypothesise:

H3: Social norms have a positive effect on the intention to slow down when driving in a koala populated area.

Perceived barriers are another important construct in SCT frameworks. There are two types of barriers: environmental and personal. Environmental barriers are those that are beyond one’s direct control. In our study, perceived barriers refer to an individual’s perceptions of the perceived hassle and/or inconvenience experienced when required to slow down at wildlife warning road signs. Within social marketing, perceived barriers are akin to negative attitudes toward an object or behaviour (Rundle-Thiele et al. 2016). For example, perceived barriers are significantly linked to road accidents among motorcyclists (Özkan et al. 2012). Therefore, we argue that strong perceived barriers will have a negative effect on behavioural intention. The hypothesis is as below:

H4: Perceived barriers have a negative effect on the intention to slow down when driving in a koala populated area.

WVO has been found to play an important role in explaining individual variation in wildlife-related behaviour and/or behavioural intention (Teel and Manfredo 2010). Studies have shown that WVO can influence attitude, behaviour and/or behavioural intention (Fulton et al. 1996; Vaske and Donnelly 1999). Miller et al. (2018), for example, found that WVO increased the effectiveness of conservation messages. In another context, Gamborg et al. (2019) applied the framework to predict landowners’ hunting management practices in rural landscapes. A model mapping the different WVO groups based on the SCT constructs may help identify differences in groups’ perceptions, enabling a more targeted approach to interventions and thus increasing their effectiveness (Miller et al. 2018). Therefore, this study proposes the following.

H5: WVO influences each of the effects between variables of the SCT model.

See Fig. 1 for the proposed model to be tested in this study.


Fig. 1.  The proposed research model.
Click to zoom


Methods

Data collection

Intercept surveys, using convenience sampling, were conducted in person from September to October in 2019. Redland City in south-east Queensland is heavily populated with koalas and has been identified by the Queensland Government as a priority koala conservation area. High foot-traffic locations in this shire such as public parks, shopping malls, bus stops, and ferry wharves were targeted for data collection. Ethics approval for the study was granted by the Griffith University Human Research Ethics committee, and a consent form was collected from each participant prior to them completing the survey (ethics number: GU Ref No: 2020/589). Two screening questions asked whether the respondent had a driving license and whether they had driven in the Ormiston and Thornlands areas in the previous 3 months. A total of 661 residents of the local area (Ormiston and Thornland suburbs) then completed a survey that gathered demographic and psychographic information on participants.

All questions in the survey required self-report answers, including demographic variables such as gender, age and educational background; and psychographic variables measuring WVO (Teel and Manfredo 2010) and SCT constructs (Bandura 1989). For the WVO scales, participants were asked about their response towards the two dimensions (mutualism and domination). SCT constructs were measured using previously validated 7 point Likert scales from Pang et al. (2018) for attitudes and intentions to drive more slowly in koala areas, Fielding et al. (2008) for both injunctive and descriptive norm, and Ayotte et al. (2010) for perceived barriers, namely hassle and increased travel time. Finally, four items measured outcome expectancies, which captured expectations about wildlife fatality and environmental conservation (Lent et al. 2017).

Data analysis

The data analysis was conducted in four steps. First, IBM SPSS ver. 26 was used to analyse descriptive statistics for the sample. Second, reliability and validity tests on WVO, attitudes and outcome expectancies were performed using Cronbach’s alpha in SPSS. Confirmatory factor analysis was performed in AMOS, following the method by Hu and Bentler (1999). Frequencies and descriptive statistics were calculated for the WVO scales to segment the sample based on an ideological matrix using a cross-tabulation procedure (Fulton et al. 1996). Finally, following the process used in Pang et al. (2018), multi-group structural equation modelling was performed in AMOS, using the SCT and WVO frameworks to investigate which factors were influencing the intention to slow down when driving in a highly populated koala area across the four WVO segments.


Results

Sample characteristics

Table 1 shows the demographic characteristic of the respondents. The sample consisted of 37.5% male and 62.5% female. Almost all (95.6%) respondents were over 18 years of age and the numbers of respondents in each age group were very similar. Over 42% of respondents had attended university.


Table 1.  Sample characteristic.
T1

Measurement model

Reliability and validity analyses were conducted to assess the adequacy of the measurement model. Reliability tests indicated that all scales had acceptable or high internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha 0.68–0.92) (Nunally and Bernstein 1978). Confirmatory factor analysis of WVOs suggested an acceptable measurement model fit (chi-square P = 0.000, chi-square/d.f. = 6.653, Comparative Fit Index (CFI) = 0.918, Tucker–Lewis Index (TLI) = 0.873, Normed Fit Index (NFI) = 0.905, Root Mean Square Error (RMSEA) = 0.093). Indicator reliability was determined from loadings, which suggested an acceptable degree of individual item reliability of 0.5 (Nunally and Bernstein 1978). Convergent validity was assessed through composite reliability (CR) and average variance extracted (AVE) (Fornell and Larcker 1981; Bagozzi and Yi 1988). The use of wildlife showed reasonably low AVE (0.474), falling below the recommended value of 0.5 (Hair 2011). The CR values for use and hunting (0.394 and 0.477) also fell below recommended levels. All other variables showed good validity (see Table 2).


Table 2.  Results of the measurement model.
Click to zoom

Wildlife value orientation types

Four WVO types were identified based on whether people scored high or low on domination and mutualism orientations (low–high, low–low, high–low, high–high) (see Table 3). They were mutualists (38.4%), distanced (14.7%), traditionalists (27.1%), and pluralists (19.8%).


Table 3.  Scoring of Koala value-orientation types on belief dimension and value-orientation scales.
Click to zoom

Structural model

The initial hypothesised model was estimated following a theory-based approach. Each SCT construct was included as a direct factor of behavioural intention. Pathways between each construct and intention referred to a hypothesis of this study. The overall fit for the model was poor (chi-square = 0.000, chi-square/d.f. = 9.662, CFI = 0.879, TLI = 0.847, Relative Fit Index (RFI) = 0.832, NFI = 0.868, RMSEA = 0.115). Only the CFI, TLI, RFI and NFI values met criteria for acceptable fit. Using the post hoc modification method recommended by Byrne (2016), modification indices indicated that deleting descriptive norms and hassle would significantly improve model fit. Therefore, the model was re-estimated, and the adjusted model showed a good fit (chi-square/d.f. = 2.824, CFI = 0.982, TLI = 0.975, RFI = 0.962, NFI = 0.972, RMSEA = 0.053). See Fig. 2 for the overall model specifications.


Fig. 2.  The validated model – overall.
Click to zoom

In the final model, there were significant pathways between intention and outcome expectancies (beta = 0.694, P < 0.001), injunctive norms (beta = 0.070, P < 0.05) and perceived barriers (beta = −0.075, P < 0.05). Therefore, H1 was supported, and H3 and H4 were partially supported. The pathway between intention and attitude was not significant (beta = 0.046, P = 0.185). Thus, H2 was not supported. Finally, a significant pathway between outcome expectancies and attitude (beta = 0.512, P < 0.001) was identified. The entire model (including all hypotheses) accounted for 39.8% of the variance explained in terms of the intention to slow down.

Multi-group analysis

The model was then tested using multi-group analysis to examine the moderating effect of WVO on the intention to slow down when driving in koala populous areas. The effects (std. beta) of each relationship in the model are shown in Table 4. The results indicated that outcome expectations were the most significant factor influencing people’s intention to protect koalas, regardless of which wildlife value they held. Outcome expectations included the expectation of protecting wildlife, reducing roadkill and keeping roads safe. Differences between groups were evident (chi-square P = 0.017) To be more exact, injunctive norms were important, positively and significantly influencing the intention of traditionalists and pluralists (but not mutualists or distanced individuals) to slow down. In addition, individual attitude positively influenced driving intention for pluralists, while perceived barriers (increased travel time) negatively influenced mutualists’ intention to slow down. Thus, H5 was supported.


Table 4.  Model path coefficient analysis and multigroup path analysis.
Click to zoom


Discussion

This study responds to the call for a better understanding of human dimensions in the discipline of conservation (Bennett et al. 2017). The study employed a widely used behavioural explanatory framework – the SCT – to investigate the determinants of behaviour change in the context of slowing down when driving to protect wildlife. The inclusion of WVO brings an additional layer of understanding, permitting group differences to be targeted based on wildlife values. This paper advances understanding in three ways. Firstly, the identification of group differences offers a practical contribution by demonstrating that different approaches are required to deliver behavioural change interventions that will benefit koalas. Secondly, this paper offers a theoretical contribution by demonstrating the value of SCT in explaining intentions to slow down to protect koalas. Finally, a methodological contribution can be offered by delivering a process that can be applied to identify values-based segments. This can guide program planning and intervention design by focussing on factors known to decrease driving speed. Each contribution is discussed in turn.

Identifying group differences to inform program planning and design

This paper combined the application of a behavioural change theoretical framework and identified group differences using the WVO framework. This has provided a comprehensive understanding of how slowing-down behaviour can be motivated in different driver groups. Segmentation challenges the fundamental mindset that a non-targeted, ‘one size fits all’ approach to behaviour change can reduce intervention effectiveness. The results of this study highlight the nuanced understanding that can emerge when group differences are identified and considered (see Table 4).

Intervention targeting strategies

We identified group differences and presented strategies to target each group, with an aim to increase conservation intervention effectiveness in changing human behaviour. The research showed that mutualists’ intentions are significantly influenced by their perceived barriers. Therefore, social marketers targeting mutualists might focus on reducing barriers to slowing down by delivering VMS indicating that travel time will not be significantly affected by slowing down at koala signs (e.g. ‘A slight delay in your travel can make a big impact on koala conservation’). Traditionalists, on the other hand, are influenced by the actions and values of friends and family. Hence, messaging that highlights family approval for slowing down around koalas may be an effective tool to target this group. Another social media communication campaign may draw on one’s own network of family and friends agreeing to slow down in koala populated areas. For example, a campaign on social media can encourage this group to slow down in koala areas by saying ‘Your loved ones want you to slow down, so do the koalas’. A strategy focused on changing attitudes is needed when targeting pluralists, as their intentions to slow down in koala areas are much more strongly influenced by their underlying wildlife attitudes, compared with other segments in the study. Therefore, messages reinforcing beliefs that slowing down can protect koalas may be an effective strategy for this group. The differences between WVO groups may be explained by differences in psychographic and demographic variables. Research indicates each group comes from a different demographic background, which accounts for differences in influential constructs. For example, Teel and Manfredo (2010) found that traditionalists and pluralists are mostly males, and older than mutualists and those who are distanced. Conservation campaigns and interventions may benefit from approaches used in other well-established sectors such as health communication, where targeting and segmentation are applied more frequently, with evidence of effectiveness (Noar et al. 2007). Similarly, research evaluating road safety campaigns suggests that segmentation techniques increase campaign effectiveness in changing road-related behaviour (Batool and Carsten 2018). Identifying the different WVO groups within an audience, along with their demographic backgrounds, will help in crafting persuasive wildlife conservation messaging and designing effective interventions.

Applying a theoretical lens to environmental protection

Set within the context of koala protection by aiming to reduce roadkill, this study covers the research need to embrace human dimensions in environmental protection efforts (Robinson et al. 2019; Veríssimo and Wan 2019). Cognitions and associations result from lifelong learning and manifest themselves in enduring dispositions to behave (Dutta-Bergman 2003). By understanding how different people think and feel we can identify factors that can be modified to reduce negative human–wildlife interactions. This human-centred approach to conservation demonstrates the application of four social marketing principles delivering a theoretically informed understanding of SCT factors that can be modified to protect koalas through driver actions. This confirms previous findings systematically linking attitudes, intentions, and other cognitive and sociodemographic variables to driving behaviour. Road safety literature utilises attitudes, social norms, and other perceptions to predict driving behaviour and behavioural intentions (Plant et al. 2017; Batool and Carsten 2018; Yousef et al. 2021). In this study, outcome expectancies, perceived barriers and social norms interacted to influence driving speed intentions. This paper demonstrates the utility of the SCT framework, which has been deemed the most used behavioural explanatory model in social marketing (Luca and Suggs 2013). It focuses on behavioural intentions in a conservation context – slowing down on the road to protect wildlife – and demonstrates the utility of the tested framework with 40% of variance explained.

Developing a more nuanced understanding: a process to identify group differences

One major threat to koalas is vehicle strike. This study, drawing on four key social marketing principles (consumer orientation, segmentation, theory, and insight) aimed to identify modifiable factors that increase people’s intention to be more vigilant in their driving behaviour in areas where koalas are present. Outcome expectations were a significant factor influencing intentions to protect koalas no matter what wildlife value a person holds, which offers one modifiable factor to target for change in a ‘one-size-fits-all’ approach. This paper delivers further evidence demonstrating how segmentation can be applied to identify segment level strategies for targeted program implementation.


Limitations and future research directions

The major limitation of this paper is that this study adopted a self-reporting survey mechanism to collect data, which may have resulted in social desirability biases (Nederhof 1985), comparing with other more advanced biometric methods to collect objective data on people’s psychographic responses such as eye tracking, facial expression detection, and brain activities via brain recorded through EEG (electroencephalography). Furthermore, the survey did not screen participants’ driving license status (Learner, Provisional, or Open licences), ownership of a vehicle, or the type of vehicle driven, to minimise the time required to complete the survey, which may have led to limited understanding of the study sample. Future research is recommended to deliver field trials to examine the effectiveness of different approaches to extend understanding of outcomes achieved and cost-effectiveness of the relative approaches.


Conclusion

In response to calls to consider human dimensions in wildlife protection, this study – set within the context of slowing down in areas where koalas are present – provides a framework that can be applied to deliver segment-level insights. This study is a first attempt to combine WVO and SCT, delivering segment-level insights to understand which modifiable factors to target in order to encourage reduction of speed in koala areas. This paper demonstrates how a nuanced understanding of human dimensions can emerge when the principle of segmentation is applied, confirming previous findings (Miller et al. 2018). The study has also generated new insights along with theoretical, methodological and practical implications. Practically, this study serves as a first step for the development of conservation efforts aiming to reduce driver speed in koala–human cohabitation zones. The insights from this study can be used to inform program design.


Data availability

The data that support this study will be shared upon reasonable request to the corresponding author.


Conflicts of interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.


Declaration of funding

The author(s) received no financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.



References

Ajzen I, Fishbein M (2005) The influence of attitudes on behavior. In ‘The handbook of attitudes’. (Eds D Albarracín, BT Johnson, MP Zanna) pp. 173–221. (Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers: Mahwah, NJ, US)

Ayotte, BJ, Margrett, JA, and Hicks-Patrick, J (2010). Physical activity in middle-aged and young-old adults: the roles of self-efficacy, barriers, outcome expectancies, self-regulatory behaviors and social support. Journal of Health Psychology 15, 173–185.
Physical activity in middle-aged and young-old adults: the roles of self-efficacy, barriers, outcome expectancies, self-regulatory behaviors and social support.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Bagozzi, RP, and Yi, Y (1988). On the evaluation of structural equation models. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science 16, 74–94.
On the evaluation of structural equation models.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Bandura, A (1989). Human agency in social cognitive theory. American Psychologist 44, 1175–1184.
Human agency in social cognitive theory.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Batool, Z, and Carsten, O (2018). Attitudinal segmentaion of drivers in Pakistan: the potential for effective road safety campaigns. Accident Analysis & Prevention 114, 48–54.
Attitudinal segmentaion of drivers in Pakistan: the potential for effective road safety campaigns.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Beedell, J, and Rehman, T (2000). Using social-psychology models to understand farmers’ conservation behaviour. Journal of Rural Studies 16, 117–127.
Using social-psychology models to understand farmers’ conservation behaviour.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Bennett, NJ, Roth, R, Klain, SC, Chan, K, Christie, P, Clark, DA, Cullman, G, Curran, D, Durbin, TJ, Epstein, G, Greenberg, A, Nelson, MP, Sandlos, J, Stedman, R, Teel, TL, Thomas, R, Veríssimo, D, and Wyborn, C (2017). Conservation social science: understanding and integrating human dimensions to improve conservation. Biological Conservation 205, 93–108.
Conservation social science: understanding and integrating human dimensions to improve conservation.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Byrne BM (2016) ‘Structural Equation Modelling with AMOS: Basic Concepts, Applications, and Programming.’ 3rd edn. (Routledge: New York)

Chin, H, Zabihi, H, Park, S, Yi, MY, and Lee, U (2017). Watchout: facilitating safe driving behaviors with social support. , .

Dahana, WD, Kobayashi, T, and Ebisuya, A (2018). Empirical study of heterogeneous behavior at the base of the pyramid: the influence of demographic and psychographic factors. Journal of International Consumer Marketing 30, 173–191.
Empirical study of heterogeneous behavior at the base of the pyramid: the influence of demographic and psychographic factors.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

David, P, Rundle-Thiele, S, Pang, B, Knox, K, Parkinson, J, and Hussenoeder, F (2019). Engaging the dog owner community in the design of an effective koala aversion program. Social Marketing Quarterly 25, 55–68.
Engaging the dog owner community in the design of an effective koala aversion program.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Department of Agriculture Water and the Environment (2022) National recovery plan for the koala Phascolarctos cinereus (combined populations of Queensland, New South Wales and the Australian Capital Territory). (Australian Government)

Department of Environment and Science (2020) Koala threats and how to help. (Department of Environment and Science)

Department of Planning, Industry and Environment (2020) Wildlife vehicle strike and contributing factors. (EaS Environment, Department of Planning, Industry and Environment: Locked Bag 5022, Parramatta, NSW 2124)

Dutta-Bergman, MJ (2003). A descriptive narrative of healthy eating: a social marketing approach using psychographics in conjunction with interpersonal, community, mass media and new media activities. Health Marketing Quarterly 20, 81–101.
A descriptive narrative of healthy eating: a social marketing approach using psychographics in conjunction with interpersonal, community, mass media and new media activities.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Fielding, KS, McDonald, R, and Louis, WR (2008). Theory of planned behaviour, identity and intentions to engage in environmental activism. Journal of Environmental Psychology 28, 318–326.
Theory of planned behaviour, identity and intentions to engage in environmental activism.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Fornell C, Larcker DF (1981) ‘Structural equation models with unobservable variables and measurement error: algebra and statistics.’ (Sage Publications Sage CA: Los Angeles, CA)

French J (2017) The importance of segmentation in social marketing strategy. In ‘Segmentation in social marketing’. (Eds T Dietrich, S Rundle-Thiele, K Kubacki) pp. 25–40. (Springer)

French, J, and Russell-Bennett, R (2015). A hierarchical model of social marketing. Journal of Social Marketing 5, 139–159.
A hierarchical model of social marketing.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Fulton, DC, Manfredo, MJ, and Lipscomb, J (1996). Wildlife value orientations: a conceptual and measurement approach. Human Dimensions of Wildlife 1, 24–47.
Wildlife value orientations: a conceptual and measurement approach.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Gamborg, C, Lund, JF, and Jensen, FS (2019). Landowners’ wildlife value orientations, attitudes and behaviour in relation to game management practices. European Journal of Wildlife Research 65, 9.
Landowners’ wildlife value orientations, attitudes and behaviour in relation to game management practices.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Glanz K (2001) Current theoretical bases for nutrition intervention and their uses. In ‘Nutrition in the prevention and treatment of disease’. (Eds AM Coulston, CL Rock, ER Monsen) pp. 83–93. (Academic Press)

Gonzalez-Astudillo V (2019) Analysis of morbidity and mortality of wild koalas in south-east queensland using passive surveillance data. PhD Thesis, School of Veterinary Science, The University of Queensland.

Hair JF (2011) Multivariate data analysis: an overview. In ‘International encyclopedia of statistical science’. (Ed. M Lovric) pp. 904–907. (Springer)

Hatchett, A, Hallam, JS, and Ford, MA (2013). Evaluation of a social cognitive theory-based email intervention designed to influence the physical activity of survivors of breast cancer. Psycho-Oncology 22, 829–836.
Evaluation of a social cognitive theory-based email intervention designed to influence the physical activity of survivors of breast cancer.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Hinsley, A, Verissimo, D, and Roberts, DL (2015). Heterogeneity in consumer preferences for orchids in international trade and the potential for the use of market research methods to study demand for wildlife. Biological Conservation 190, 80–86.
Heterogeneity in consumer preferences for orchids in international trade and the potential for the use of market research methods to study demand for wildlife.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Homer, PM, and Kahle, LR (1988). A structural equation test of the value-attitude-behavior hierarchy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 54, 638–646.
A structural equation test of the value-attitude-behavior hierarchy.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Hrubes, D, Ajzen, I, and Daigle, J (2001). Predicting hunting intentions and behavior: an application of the theory of planned behavior. Leisure Sciences 23, 165–178.
Predicting hunting intentions and behavior: an application of the theory of planned behavior.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Hu, L-t, and Bentler, PM (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal 6, 1–55.
Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

International Fund for Animal Welfare (2022) Koala conservation status in New South Wales. (International Fund for Animal Welfare)

Jones, S, Keane, A, St John, F, Vickery, J, and Papworth, S (2019). Audience segmentation to improve targeting of conservation interventions for hunters. Conservation Biology 33, 895–905.
Audience segmentation to improve targeting of conservation interventions for hunters.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Kubacki, K, Ronto, R, Lahtinen, V, Pang, B, and Rundle-Thiele, S (2017a). Social marketing interventions aiming to increase physical activity among adults: a systematic review. Health Education 117, 69–89.
Social marketing interventions aiming to increase physical activity among adults: a systematic review.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Kubacki K, Rundle-Thiele S, Pang B, Carins J, Parkinson J, Fujihira H, Ronto R (2017b) An umbrella review of the use of segmentation in social marketing interventions. In ‘Segmentation in Social Marketing’. (Eds T Dietrich, S Rundle-Thiele, K Kubacki) pp. 9–23. (Springer)

Lent, RW, Ireland, GW, Penn, LT, Morris, TR, and Sappington, R (2017). Sources of self-efficacy and outcome expectations for career exploration and decision-making: a test of the social cognitive model of career self-management. Journal of Vocational Behavior 99, 107–117.
Sources of self-efficacy and outcome expectations for career exploration and decision-making: a test of the social cognitive model of career self-management.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Luca, NR, and Suggs, LS (2013). Theory and model use in social marketing health interventions. Journal of Health Communication 18, 20–40.
Theory and model use in social marketing health interventions.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Lunney, D, Predavec, M, Sonawane, I, Moon, C, and Rhodes, JR (2022). Factors that drive koala roadkill: an analysis across multiple scales in New South Wales, Australia. Australian Mammalogy 44, 328–337.
Factors that drive koala roadkill: an analysis across multiple scales in New South Wales, Australia.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Manfredo, MJ, Teel, TL, and Henry, KL (2009). Linking society and environment: a multilevel model of shifting wildlife value orientations in the western United States. Social Science Quarterly 90, 407–427.
Linking society and environment: a multilevel model of shifting wildlife value orientations in the western United States.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

McAlpine, C, Lunney, D, Melzer, A, Menkhorst, P, Phillips, S, Phalen, D, Ellis, W, Foley, W, Baxter, G, de Villiers, D, Kavanagh, R, Adams-Hosking, C, Todd, C, Whisson, D, Molsher, R, Walter, M, Lawler, I, and Close, R (2015). Conserving koalas: a review of the contrasting regional trends, outlooks and policy challenges. Biological Conservation 192, 226–236.
Conserving koalas: a review of the contrasting regional trends, outlooks and policy challenges.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

McKenzie-Mohr D, Lee NR, Schultz PW, Kotler P (2012) ‘Social marketing to protect the environment: what works.’ (SAGE publications)

Mehta, P, and Chahal, HS (2021). Consumer attitude towards green products: revisiting the profile of green consumers using segmentation approach. Management of Environmental Quality 32, 902–928.
Consumer attitude towards green products: revisiting the profile of green consumers using segmentation approach.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Metcalf, AL, Angle, JW, Phelan, CN, Muth, BA, and Finley, JC (2019). More “bank” for the buck: microtargeting and normative appeals to increase social marketing efficiency. Social Marketing Quarterly 25, 26–39.
More “bank” for the buck: microtargeting and normative appeals to increase social marketing efficiency.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Miller, ZD, Freimund, W, Metcalf, EC, and Nickerson, N (2018). Targeting your audience: wildlife value orientations and the relevance of messages about bear safety. Human Dimensions of Wildlife 23, 213–226.
Targeting your audience: wildlife value orientations and the relevance of messages about bear safety.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Nederhof, AJ (1985). Methods of coping with social desirability bias: a review. European Journal of Social Psychology 15, 263–280.
Methods of coping with social desirability bias: a review.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Noar, SM, Benac, CN, and Harris, MS (2007). Does tailoring matter? Meta-analytic review of tailored print health behavior change interventions. Psychological Bulletin 133, 673–693.
Does tailoring matter? Meta-analytic review of tailored print health behavior change interventions.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

NSW Parliament Enquiry (2020) Koala populations and habitat in New South Wales. (NSW Parliament Enquiry)

Nunally JC, Bernstein I (1978) ‘Psychometric theory.’ (McGraw-Hill: New York)

Özkan, T, Lajunen, T, Doğruyol, B, Yıldırım, Z, and Çoymak, A (2012). Motorcycle accidents, rider behaviour, and psychological models. Accident Analysis & Prevention 49, 124–132.
Motorcycle accidents, rider behaviour, and psychological models.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Pang, B, Rundle-Thiele, S, and Kubacki, K (2018). Can the theory of planned behaviour explain walking to and from school among Australian children? A social marketing formative research study. International Journal of Nonprofit and Voluntary Sector Marketing 23, e1599.
Can the theory of planned behaviour explain walking to and from school among Australian children? A social marketing formative research study.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Plant, BRC, Irwin, JD, and Chekaluk, E (2017). The effects of anti-speeding advertisements on the simulated driving behaviour of young drivers. Accident Analysis & Prevention 100, 65–74.
The effects of anti-speeding advertisements on the simulated driving behaviour of young drivers.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Plotnikoff, RC, Costigan, SA, Karunamuni, N, and Lubans, DR (2013). Social cognitive theories used to explain physical activity behavior in adolescents: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Preventive Medicine 56, 245–253.
Social cognitive theories used to explain physical activity behavior in adolescents: a systematic review and meta-analysis.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Queensland Department of Environment and Science (2019) Koala threats: koala on roads. (Queensland Department of Environment and Science)

Rakotonirainy, A, Schroeter, R, and Soro, A (2014). Three social car visions to improve driver behaviour. Pervasive and Mobile Computing 14, 147–160.
Three social car visions to improve driver behaviour.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Redland City Council (2016) Redlands koala conservation action plan 2016–2021. (Redland City Council)

Reid, AE, and Aiken, LS (2013). Correcting injunctive norm misperceptions motivates behavior change: a randomized controlled sun protection intervention. Health Psychology 32, 551–560.
Correcting injunctive norm misperceptions motivates behavior change: a randomized controlled sun protection intervention.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Robinson, BS, Creasey, MJS, Skeats, A, Coverdale, I, and Barlow, A (2019). Global survey reveals a lack of social marketing skills in the conservation sector and shows supply of training doesn’t meet demand. Social Marketing Quarterly 25, 9–25.
Global survey reveals a lack of social marketing skills in the conservation sector and shows supply of training doesn’t meet demand.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Rundle-Thiele, S, Kubacki, K, and Gruneklee, N (2016). Perceived benefits and barriers of physical activity: a social marketing formative study. Health Marketing Quarterly 33, 181–194.
Perceived benefits and barriers of physical activity: a social marketing formative study.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Rundle-Thiele, S, Pang, B, Knox, K, David, P, Parkinson, J, and Hussenoeder, F (2019a). Generating new directions for reducing dog and koala interactions: a social marketing formative research study. Australasian Journal of Environmental Management 26, 173–187.
Generating new directions for reducing dog and koala interactions: a social marketing formative research study.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Rundle-Thiele, S, David, P, Willmott, T, Pang, B, Eagle, L, and Hay, R (2019b). Social marketing theory development goals: an agenda to drive change. Journal of Marketing Management 35, 160–181.
Social marketing theory development goals: an agenda to drive change.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Sawitri, DR, Hadiyanto, H, and Hadi, SP (2015). Pro-environmental behavior from a socialcognitive theory perspective. Procedia Environmental Sciences 23, 27–33.
Pro-environmental behavior from a socialcognitive theory perspective.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Shumway, N, Lunney, D, Seabrook, L, and McAlpine, C (2015). Saving our national icon: an ecological analysis of the 2011 Australian Senate inquiry into status of the koala. Environmental Science & Policy 54, 297–303.
Saving our national icon: an ecological analysis of the 2011 Australian Senate inquiry into status of the koala.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Steel, BS, List, P, and Shindler, B (1994). Conflicting values about federal forests - a comparison of national and oregon publics. Society & Natural Resources 7, 137–153.
Conflicting values about federal forests - a comparison of national and oregon publics.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Tabernero, C, and Hernández, B (2011). Self-efficacy and intrinsic motivation guiding environmental behavior. Environment and Behavior 43, 658–675.
Self-efficacy and intrinsic motivation guiding environmental behavior.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Tang, Z, Chen, X, and Luo, J (2011). Determining socio-psychological drivers for rural household recycling behavior in developing countries: a case study from Wugan, Hunan, China. Environment and Behavior 43, 848–877.
Determining socio-psychological drivers for rural household recycling behavior in developing countries: a case study from Wugan, Hunan, China.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Teel, TL, and Manfredo, MJ (2010). Understanding the diversity of public interests in wildlife conservation. Conservation Biology 24, 128–139.
Understanding the diversity of public interests in wildlife conservation.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Trask S (2020) Bushfires hastening NSW koala extinction, The Advocate. Available at https://www.theadvocate.com.au/story/6636302/bushfires-hastening-nsw-koala-extinction/

Truong, VD (2014). Social marketing: a systematic review of research 1998–2012. Social Marketing Quarterly 20, 15–34.
Social marketing: a systematic review of research 1998–2012.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Vaske, JJ, and Donnelly, MP (1999). A value-attitude-behavior model predicting wildland preservation voting intentions. Society & Natural Resources 12, 523–537.
A value-attitude-behavior model predicting wildland preservation voting intentions.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Veríssimo, D, and Wan, AKY (2019). Characterizing efforts to reduce consumer demand for wildlife products. Conservation Biology 33, 623–633.
Characterizing efforts to reduce consumer demand for wildlife products.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Wauters, E, Bielders, C, Poesen, J, Govers, G, and Mathijs, E (2010). Adoption of soil conservation practices in Belgium: an examination of the theory of planned behaviour in the agri-environmental domain. Land Use Policy 27, 86–94.
Adoption of soil conservation practices in Belgium: an examination of the theory of planned behaviour in the agri-environmental domain.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Yousef, M, Dietrich, T, and Torrisi, G (2021). Positive, negative or both? Assessing emotional appeals effectiveness in anti-drink driving advertisements. Social Marketing Quarterly 27, 195–212.
Positive, negative or both? Assessing emotional appeals effectiveness in anti-drink driving advertisements.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |