Free Standard AU & NZ Shipping For All Book Orders Over $80!
Register      Login
Emu Emu Society
Journal of BirdLife Australia
RESEARCH ARTICLE

Chatter-call harmonics in the North Island Saddleback: do they play a role in ranging?

Joseph F. Azar A B , Ben D. Bell A and K. C. Burns A
+ Author Affiliations
- Author Affiliations

A Centre for Biodiversity & Restoration Ecology, School of Biological Sciences, Victoria University of Wellington, PO Box 600, Wellington 6041, New Zealand.

B Corresponding author. Email: azar.joseph@gmail.com

Emu 113(2) 161-167 https://doi.org/10.1071/MU12099
Submitted: 22 June 2012  Accepted: 20 November 2012   Published: 21 February 2013

Abstract

Birds that counter-sing for communication and territorial maintenance need to localise the source of sound in order to promote an appropriate intraspecific response. Here, we investigate the role of harmonics in the chatter call of the North Island Saddleback (Philesturnus rufusater). We test whether the relative amplitude of harmonics serves as a distance cue, and whether a change of the harmonic composition of the chatter call has an effect on bird’s response and its likely ability to estimate the distance of the signalling individual. North Island Saddlebacks exhibited stronger responses to playback songs with more relative energy within higher harmonics, suggesting that these are perceived as coming from a nearby individual. North Island Saddlebacks took longer to respond and counter-sang less to chatter calls with more relative energy in lower harmonics, suggesting that they were perceived as coming from a distant bird. We also found that North Island Saddlebacks responded differently to songs from which different harmonic frequencies were removed (muted). This study reveals the ability of the North Island Saddleback to differentiate between calls with different harmonic composition and proposes that harmonics are important as distance cues.

Additional keywords: bird song, Philesturnus rufusater, playback.


References

Aubin, T., and Jouventin, P. (2002). Localisation of an acoustic signal in a noisy environment: the display call of the King Penguin Aptenodytes patagonicus. Journal of Experimental Biology 205, 3793–3798.

Brenowitz, E. A. (1982). Long-range communication of species identity by song in the Red-winged Blackbird. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 10, 29–38.
Long-range communication of species identity by song in the Red-winged Blackbird.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Brumm, H., and Slater, P. J. B. (2006). Animals can vary signal amplitude with receiver distance: evidence from Zebra Finch song. Animal Behaviour 72, 699–705.
Animals can vary signal amplitude with receiver distance: evidence from Zebra Finch song.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Buchanan, K. L., and Catchpole, C. K. (1997). Female choice in the Sedge Warbler Acrocephalus schoenobaenus multiple cues from song and territory quality. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences 264, 521–526.
Female choice in the Sedge Warbler Acrocephalus schoenobaenus multiple cues from song and territory quality.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Catchpole, C. K., and Slater, P. J. B. (2008). ‘Bird Song. Biological Themes and Variations.’ (Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK.)

Catchpole, C. K., Dittami, J., and Leisler, B. (1984). Differential responses to male song repertoires in female songbirds implanted with oestradiol. Nature 312, 563–564.
Differential responses to male song repertoires in female songbirds implanted with oestradiol.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Clench, M. H. (1978). Tracheal elongation in birds-of-paradise. Condor 80, 423–430.
Tracheal elongation in birds-of-paradise.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Darwin, C. J., Ciocca, V., and Sandell, G. J. (1994). Effects of frequency and amplitude modulation on the pitch of a complex tone with a mistuned harmonic. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 95, 2631–2636.
Effects of frequency and amplitude modulation on the pitch of a complex tone with a mistuned harmonic.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 1:STN:280:DyaK2c3mslSqtw%3D%3D&md5=e8bdeacc657d63f396966737c19b6a81CAS |

Fitch, W. T. (1999). Acoustic exaggeration of size in birds via tracheal elongation: comparative and theoretical analyses. Journal of Zoology 248, 31–48.
Acoustic exaggeration of size in birds via tracheal elongation: comparative and theoretical analyses.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Fitch, W. T., and Hauser, M. D. (2003). Unpacking ‘honesty’: vertebrate vocal production and the evolution of acoustic signals. In ‘Acoustic Communication’. (Eds A. M. Simmons, A. N. Popper and R. R. Fay.) pp. 65–137. (Springer: New York.)

Fitch, W. T., and Kelley, J. P. (2000). Perception of vocal tract resonances by Whooping Cranes Grus americana. Ethology 106, 559–574.
Perception of vocal tract resonances by Whooping Cranes Grus americana.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Fotheringham, J. R., Martin, P. R., and Ratcliffe, L. (1997). Song transmission and auditory perception of distance in Wood Warblers (Parulinae). Animal Behaviour 53, 1271–1285.
Song transmission and auditory perception of distance in Wood Warblers (Parulinae).Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Goller, F., Mallinckrodt, M. J., and Torti, S. D. (2004). Beak gape dynamics during song in the Zebra Finch. Journal of Neurobiology 59, 289–303.
Beak gape dynamics during song in the Zebra Finch.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Handel, S. (1995). Timbre perception and auditory object identification. In ‘Hearing’. (Editor: B. C. J. Moore.) pp. 425–461. (Academic Press: San Diego, CA.)

Jenkins, P. F. (1978). Cultural transmission of song patterns and dialect development in a free-living bird population. Animal Behaviour 26, 50–78.
Cultural transmission of song patterns and dialect development in a free-living bird population.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Krebs, J., Ashcroft, R., and Webber, M. (1978). Song repertoires and territory defence in the Great Tit. Nature 271, 539–542.
Song repertoires and territory defence in the Great Tit.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Kroodsma, D., Byers, B., Goodale, E., Johnson, S., and Liu, W. (2001). Pseudoreplication in playback experiments, revisited a decade later. Animal Behaviour 61, 1029–1033.
Pseudoreplication in playback experiments, revisited a decade later.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Littel, R. C., Freund, R. J., and Spector, P. C. (1991). ‘SAS System for Linear Models.’ (SAS Institute Inc.: Cary, NC.)

Lohr, B., and Dooling, R. J. (1998). Detection of changes in timbre and harmonicity in complex sounds by Zebra Finches (Taeniopygia guttata) and Budgerigars (Melopsittacus undulatus). Journal of Comparative Psychology 112, 36–47.
Detection of changes in timbre and harmonicity in complex sounds by Zebra Finches (Taeniopygia guttata) and Budgerigars (Melopsittacus undulatus).Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 1:STN:280:DyaK1c7ovVGntA%3D%3D&md5=c20df9bd4dbf7791c520df5ff63df8bbCAS |

Lord, K., Feinstein, M., and Coppinger, R. (2009). Barking and mobbing. Behavioural Processes 81, 358–368.
Barking and mobbing.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Ludwig, K., and Jamieson, I. G. (2007). Phrase types, repertoire size and repertoire overlap in the South Island Saddleback (Philesturnus carunculatus carunculatus). Notornis 54, 201–213.

Marion, W. R. (1977). Growth and development of the Plain Chachalaca in South Texas. Wilson Bulletin 89, 47–56.

May, B., Moody, D. B., and Stebbins, W. C. (1989). Categorical perception of conspecific communication sounds by Japanese Macaques, Macaca fuscata. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 85, 837–847.
Categorical perception of conspecific communication sounds by Japanese Macaques, Macaca fuscata.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 1:STN:280:DyaL1M7nsFOltw%3D%3D&md5=8e6eac87fe17bc23a54ba2bd9e6f2674CAS |

McGregor, P. K., and Krebs, J. R. (1984). Sound degradation as a distance cue in Great Tit (Parus major) song. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 16, 49–56.
Sound degradation as a distance cue in Great Tit (Parus major) song.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

McGregor, P. K., Krebs, J. R., and Ratcliffe, L. M. (1983). The reaction of Great Tits (Parus major) to playback of degraded and undergraded songs: the effect of familiarity with the stimulus song type. Auk 100, 898–906.

Naguib, M. (1995). Auditory distance assessment of singing conspecifies in Carolina Wrens: the role of reverberation and frequency-dependent attenuation. Animal Behaviour 50, 1297–1307.
Auditory distance assessment of singing conspecifies in Carolina Wrens: the role of reverberation and frequency-dependent attenuation.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Naguib, M. (1996). Auditory distance estimation in song birds: implications, methodologies and perspectives. Behavioural Processes 38, 163–168.
Auditory distance estimation in song birds: implications, methodologies and perspectives.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Naguib, M. (1997a). Ranging of songs in Carolina Wrens: effects of familiarity with the song type on use of different cues. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 40, 385–393.
Ranging of songs in Carolina Wrens: effects of familiarity with the song type on use of different cues.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Naguib, M. (1997b). Use of song amplitude for ranging in Carolina Wrens, Thryothorus ludovicianus. Ethology 103, 723–731.
Use of song amplitude for ranging in Carolina Wrens, Thryothorus ludovicianus.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Naguib, M., and Wiley, R. H. (2001). Estimating the distance to a source of sound: mechanisms and adaptations for long-range communication. Animal Behaviour 62, 825–837.
Estimating the distance to a source of sound: mechanisms and adaptations for long-range communication.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Naguib, M., Klump, G. M., Hillmann, E., Griessmann, B., and Teige, T. (2000). Assessment of auditory distance in a territorial songbird: accurate feat or rule of thumb? Animal Behaviour 59, 715–721.
Assessment of auditory distance in a territorial songbird: accurate feat or rule of thumb?Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Nelson, B. S. (2000). Avian dependence on sound pressure level as an auditory distance cue. Animal Behaviour 59, 57–67.
Avian dependence on sound pressure level as an auditory distance cue.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Padgham, M. (2004). Reverberation and frequency attenuation in forests – implications for acoustic communication in animals. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 115, 402–410.
Reverberation and frequency attenuation in forests – implications for acoustic communication in animals.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Parker, K. A., Hauber, M. E., and Brunton, D. H. (2010). Contemporary cultural evolution of a conspecific recognition signal following serial translocations. Evolution 64, 2431–2441.

Podos, J., Sherer, J. K., Peters, S., and Nowicki, S. (1995). Ontogeny of vocal tract movements during song production in Song Sparrows. Animal Behaviour 50, 1287–1296.
Ontogeny of vocal tract movements during song production in Song Sparrows.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Richards, D. G. (1981). Estimation of distance of singing conspecifics by the Carolina Wren. Auk 98, 127–133.

Slabbekoorn, H., Ellers, J., and Smith, T. B. (2002). Birdsong and sound transmission: the benefits of reverberations. Condor 104, 564–573.
Birdsong and sound transmission: the benefits of reverberations.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Slater, P. J. B. (2003). Fifty years of bird song research: a case study in animal behaviour. Animal Behaviour 65, 633–639.
Fifty years of bird song research: a case study in animal behaviour.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Weiss, D. J., and Hauser, M. D. (2002). Perception of harmonics in the combination long call of Cottontop Tamarins, Saguinus oedipus. Animal Behaviour 64, 415–426.
Perception of harmonics in the combination long call of Cottontop Tamarins, Saguinus oedipus.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Westneat, M. W., Long, J. H., Hoese, W., and Nowicki, S. (1993). Kinematics of birdsong: functional correlation of cranial movements and acoustic features in sparrows. Journal of Experimental Biology 182, 147–171.
| 1:STN:280:DyaK2c%2FksFCjuw%3D%3D&md5=bcbdc6136f6cc989f4065d05cb66d242CAS |

Wiley, R. H., and Godard, R. (1996). Ranging of conspecific songs by Kentucky Warblers and its implications for interactions of territorial males. Behaviour 133, 81–102.
Ranging of conspecific songs by Kentucky Warblers and its implications for interactions of territorial males.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |