Register      Login
Emu Emu Society
Journal of BirdLife Australia
RESEARCH ARTICLE

Variation in the alarm calls of Southern House Wrens (Troglodytes musculus)

Gustavo J. Fernández A B , M. Gabriela Corral A and Mariana E. Carro A
+ Author Affiliations
- Author Affiliations

A Laboratorio de Ecología y Comportamiento Animal, Departamento de Ecología, Genética y Evolución, Facultad de Ciencias Exactas y Naturales, Universidad de Buenos Aires, Pabellón II Ciudad Universitaria, C1428EHA, Buenos Aires, Argentina.

B Corresponding author. Email: gjf@ege.fcen.uba.ar

Emu 112(1) 71-75 https://doi.org/10.1071/MU11033
Submitted: 27 April 2011  Accepted: 31 October 2011   Published: 15 February 2012

Abstract

We examined variation in the structure and characteristics of alarm calls of nesting Southern House Wrens (Troglodytes musculus) during the 2007 and 2008 breeding seasons. We took multiple measures of 221 calls of 21 males and 18 females and analysed them for individual and sexual differences. We recognised two distinctive alarm calls, named Type I (TI) and Type II (TII). Sexes did not differ in rates of calling or call structure. However, females produced TI calls more frequently and TII calls less frequently than males. Rate and acoustical structure of TI calls varied with the age of nestlings, but there was no relationship between this factor and TII calls. Individuals increased the emission of TI calls with nestling age, but these calls had lower frequencies when nestlings were older. Given the differences we found across call types, we propose that each call type has a different function, which remains to be tested.

Additional keywords: calling rate, call structure, communication, signal function.


References

Blumstein, D. T., and Munos, O. (2005). Individual and age/sex class variation in marmot alarm calls. Animal Behaviour 69, 353–361.
Individual and age/sex class variation in marmot alarm calls.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Blumstein, D. T., Verenyre, L., and Daniel, J. C. (2004). Reliability and the adaptive utility of discrimination among alarm callers. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences 271, 1851–1857.
Reliability and the adaptive utility of discrimination among alarm callers.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Burger, J., Gochfeld, M., Saliva, J. E., Gochfeld, D., and Morales, H. (1989). Antipredator behaviour in nesting Zenaida Doves (Zenaida aurita): parental investment or offspring vulnerability. Behaviour 111, 129–143.
Antipredator behaviour in nesting Zenaida Doves (Zenaida aurita): parental investment or offspring vulnerability.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Cheney, D. L., and Seyfarth, R. M. (1988). Assessment of meaning and the detection of unreliable signals by vervet monkeys. Animal Behaviour 36, 477–486.
Assessment of meaning and the detection of unreliable signals by vervet monkeys.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Colombelli-Négrel, D., Robertson, J., and Kleindorfer, S. (2011). Risky revelations: superb Fairy-wrens Malurus cyaneus respond more strongly to their mate’s alarm song. Journal of Ornithology 152, 127–135.
Risky revelations: superb Fairy-wrens Malurus cyaneus respond more strongly to their mate’s alarm song.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Crawley, M. (1993). ‘Glim for Ecologists.’ (Blackwell Scientific Publications: Oxford, UK.)

Curio, E. (1978). The adaptive significance of avian mobbing: I. Teleonomic hypothesis and predictions. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie 48, 175–183.

Curio, E. (1987). Brood defence in the Great Tit: the influence of age, number and quality of young. Ardea 75, 35–42.

Fasanella, M., and Fernández, G. J. (2009). Alarm calls of the Southern House Wren, Troglodytes musculus: variation with nesting stage and predator model. Journal of Ornithology 150, 853–863.
Alarm calls of the Southern House Wren, Troglodytes musculus: variation with nesting stage and predator model.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Greig-Smith, P. W. (1980). Parental investment in nest defence by Stonechats (Saxicola torquata). Animal Behaviour 28, 604–619.
Parental investment in nest defence by Stonechats (Saxicola torquata).Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Grim, T. (2008). Are Blackcaps (Sylvia atricapilla) defending their nests also calling for help from their neighbours? Journal of Ornithology 149, 169–180.
Are Blackcaps (Sylvia atricapilla) defending their nests also calling for help from their neighbours?Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Haftorn, S. (2000). Contexts and possible functions of alarm calling in the Willow Tit, Parus montanus: the principle of “better safe than sorry”. Behaviour 137, 437–449.
Contexts and possible functions of alarm calling in the Willow Tit, Parus montanus: the principle of “better safe than sorry”.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Hasson, O. (1991). Pursuit-deterrent signals: communication between prey and predator. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 6, 325–329.
Pursuit-deterrent signals: communication between prey and predator.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 1:STN:280:DC%2BC3M7hsVersA%3D%3D&md5=9b8c6c840c36eca05db6a5cd8c18dad6CAS |

Högstad, O. (1995). Alarm calling by Willow Tits, Parus montanus, as mate investment. Animal Behaviour 49, 221–225.
Alarm calling by Willow Tits, Parus montanus, as mate investment.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Högstedt, G. (1983). Adaptation unto death: function of fear screams. American Naturalist 121, 562–570.
Adaptation unto death: function of fear screams.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Kennedy, R. A. W., Evans, C. S., and McDonald, P. G. (2009). Individual distinctiveness in the mobbing call of a cooperative bird, the Noisy Miner Manorina melanocephala. Journal of Avian Biology 40, 481–490.
Individual distinctiveness in the mobbing call of a cooperative bird, the Noisy Miner Manorina melanocephala.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Kleindorfer, S., Hoi, H., and Fessl, B. (1996). Alarm calls and chick reactions in the Moustached Warbler, Acrocephalus melanopogon. Animal Behaviour 51, 1199–1206.
Alarm calls and chick reactions in the Moustached Warbler, Acrocephalus melanopogon.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Kleindorfer, S., Fessl, B., and Hoi, H. (2005). Avian nest defence behaviour: risk assessment in relation to predator distance and type, and nest height. Animal Behaviour 69, 307–313.
Avian nest defence behaviour: risk assessment in relation to predator distance and type, and nest height.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Klump, G. M., and Shalter, M. D. (1984). Acoustic behaviour of birds and mammals in the predator context. II. The functional significance and evolution of alarm signals. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie 66, 206–226.

Knight, R. L., and Temple, S. A. (1988). Nest defense behavior in the Red-winged Blackbird. Condor 90, 193–200.
Nest defense behavior in the Red-winged Blackbird.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Krams, I., Krama, T., and Igaune, K. (2006). Alarm calls of wintering Great Tits Parus major: warning of mate, reciprocal altruism or a message to the predator? Journal of Avian Biology 37, 131–136.
Alarm calls of wintering Great Tits Parus major: warning of mate, reciprocal altruism or a message to the predator?Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Llambías, P. E., and Fernández, G. J. (2009). Effects of nestboxes on the breeding biology of Southern House Wrens Troglodytes aedon bonariae in the southern temperate zone. Ibis 151, 113–121.
Effects of nestboxes on the breeding biology of Southern House Wrens Troglodytes aedon bonariae in the southern temperate zone.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Madden, J. R., Kilner, R. M., and Davies, N. B. (2005). Nestling responses to adult food and alarm calls: 1. Species-specific responses in two cowbird hosts. Animal Behaviour 70, 619–627.
Nestling responses to adult food and alarm calls: 1. Species-specific responses in two cowbird hosts.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Marler, P. (2004). Bird calls: their potential for behavioral neurobiology. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1016, 31–44.
Bird calls: their potential for behavioral neurobiology.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

McCowan, B., and Hooper, S. L. (2002). Individual acoustic variation in Belding’s Ground Squirrel alarm chirps in the High Sierra Nevada. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 111, 1157–1160.
Individual acoustic variation in Belding’s Ground Squirrel alarm chirps in the High Sierra Nevada.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Mundry, R., and Sommer, C. (2007). Discriminant function analysis with nonindependent data: consequences and an alternative. Animal Behaviour 74, 965–976.
Discriminant function analysis with nonindependent data: consequences and an alternative.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Platzen, D., and Magrath, R. D. (2004). Parental alarm calls suppress nestling vocalization. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences 271, 1271–1276.
Parental alarm calls suppress nestling vocalization.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Rajala, M., Ratti, O., and Suhonen, J. (2003). Age differences in the response of Willow Tits (Parus montanus) to conspecific clarm calls. Ethology 109, 501–509.
Age differences in the response of Willow Tits (Parus montanus) to conspecific clarm calls.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Redondo, T., and Carranza, J. (1989). Offspring reproductive value and nest defense in the Magpie (Pica pica). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 25, 369–378.
Offspring reproductive value and nest defense in the Magpie (Pica pica).Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Serra, C., and Fernández, G. J. (2011). Reduction of nestlings’ vocalizations in response to parental alarm calls in the Southern House Wren, Troglodytes musculus. Journal of Ornithology 152, 331–336.
Reduction of nestlings’ vocalizations in response to parental alarm calls in the Southern House Wren, Troglodytes musculus.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Suzuki, T. N. (2011). Parental alarm calls warn nestlings about different predatory threats. Current Biology 21, R15–R16.
Parental alarm calls warn nestlings about different predatory threats.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 1:CAS:528:DC%2BC3MXkt1Srtw%3D%3D&md5=57ee95d3aca3321f0a5d4be7a9908c54CAS |

Weatherhead, P. J. (1989). Nest defence by Song Sparrows: methodological and life history considerations. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 25, 129–136.
Nest defence by Song Sparrows: methodological and life history considerations.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Yasukawa, K. (1989). The costs and benefits of a vocal signal: the nest-associated ‘chit’ of the female Red-winged Blackbird, Agelaius phoeniceus. Animal Behaviour 38, 866–874.
The costs and benefits of a vocal signal: the nest-associated ‘chit’ of the female Red-winged Blackbird, Agelaius phoeniceus.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Zuberbühler, K. (2009). Survivor signals: the biology and psychology of animal alarm calling. Advances in the Study of Behavior 40, 277–322.
Survivor signals: the biology and psychology of animal alarm calling.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |