Free Standard AU & NZ Shipping For All Book Orders Over $80!
Register      Login
Emu Emu Society
Journal of BirdLife Australia
RESEARCH ARTICLE

The role of toxic pitohuis in mixed-species flocks of lowland forest in Papua New Guinea

Eben Goodale A C D , Uromi Goodale B C and Ralph Mana A
+ Author Affiliations
- Author Affiliations

A Department of Biological Sciences, School of Natural and Physical Sciences, University of Papua New Guinea, PO Box 320, University Post Office, National Capital District, Papua New Guinea.

B Yale School of Forestry and Environmental Studies, 195 Prospect Street, New Haven, CT 06511, USA.

C Present Addresses: Section of Ecology, Behavior and Evolution, Division of Biological Sciences, University of California, San Diego, 9500 Gilman Drive, La Jolla, CA 92093, USA.

D Corresponding author. Email: eben.goodale@gmail.com

Emu 112(1) 9-16 https://doi.org/10.1071/MU11026
Submitted: 26 March 2011  Accepted: 22 July 2011   Published: 9 December 2011

Abstract

In many animal taxa, non-toxic species demonstrate Batesian mimicry – appearing or behaving similarly to harmful species – thereby reducing their attractiveness to predators. A potential example of Batesian mimicry occurs in Papua New Guinea (PNG), where birds of the genus Pitohui have been found to contain toxic compounds; pitohuis are frequent members of mixed-species flocks and several observers have hypothesised that species in flocks are imitating each other in their plumage and vocalisations. If non-toxic species participate in flocks to associate with pitohuis, we predicted that pitohuis should play an important role in flocks, such that flock composition should be strongly influenced by their presence, and that other species should be attracted towards their vocalisations. We found, however, that in the lowland rainforests of Madang Province, flock composition was less influenced by the presence of pitohuis than by the presence of Rufous Babblers (Pomatostomus isidori), a non-toxic leading species. In playback experiments, Rufous Babblers were as attractive to heterospecifics as pitohuis. Our study provides the first empirical test of the connection between toxicity and flock organisation in PNG and our primarily negative results suggests that toxicity does not drive the organisation of flocks in our study area.


References

Barnard, C. J. (1979). Predation and the evolution of social mimicry in birds. American Naturalist 113, 613–618.
Predation and the evolution of social mimicry in birds.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Bates, H. W. (1862). Contributions to an insect fauna of the Amazon valley. Lepidoptera : Heliconidae. Transactions of the Linnaean Society of London 23, 495–566.
Contributions to an insect fauna of the Amazon valley. Lepidoptera : Heliconidae.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Beauchamp, G., and Goodale, E. (2011). Plumage mimicry in avian mixed-species flocks: more or less than meet the eye? Auk 128, 487–496.

Beehler, B. M., Pratt, T. K., and Zimmerman, D. A. (1986). ‘Birds of New Guinea.’ (Princeton University Press: Princeton, NJ.)

Béland, P. (1977). Mimicry in orioles in south-eastern Queensland. Emu 77, 215–218.
Mimicry in orioles in south-eastern Queensland.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Bell, H. L. (1982a). A bird community of lowland rainforest in New Guinea. 5. Mixed-species feeding flocks. Emu 82, 256–275.
A bird community of lowland rainforest in New Guinea. 5. Mixed-species feeding flocks.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Bell, H. L. (1982b). Social organization and feeding of the Rufous Babbler Pomatostomus isidori. Emu 82, 7–11.
Social organization and feeding of the Rufous Babbler Pomatostomus isidori.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Bibby, C. J., Burgess, N. D., Hill, D. A., and Mustoe, S. (1992). ‘Bird Census Techniques.’ (Academic Press: London.)

Caldwell, G. S. (1981). Attraction to tropical mixed-species heron flocks: proximate mechanism and consequences. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 8, 99–103.
Attraction to tropical mixed-species heron flocks: proximate mechanism and consequences.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Coates, B. J., and Peckover, W. S. (2001). ‘Birds of New Guinea and the Bismarck Archipelago: A Photographic Guide.’ (Dove Publications: Brisbane.)

Cody, M. L. (1970). Chilean bird distribution. Ecology 51, 455–464.
Chilean bird distribution.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Cody, M. L., and Brown, J. H. (1970). Character convergence in Mexican finches. Evolution 24, 304–310.
Character convergence in Mexican finches.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Cott, H. B. (1947). The edibility of birds: illustrated by five years’ experiments and observations (1941–1946) on the food preferences of the hornet, cat and man; and considered with special reference to the theories of adaptive coloration. Proceedings of the Zoological Society of London 116, 371–524.
The edibility of birds: illustrated by five years’ experiments and observations (1941–1946) on the food preferences of the hornet, cat and man; and considered with special reference to the theories of adaptive coloration.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Diamond, J. M. (1982). Mimicry of friarbirds by orioles. Auk 99, 187–196.

Diamond, J. (1987). Flocks of brown and black New Guinea birds: a bicoloured mixed-species foraging association. Emu 87, 201–211.
Flocks of brown and black New Guinea birds: a bicoloured mixed-species foraging association.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Diamond, J. M. (1992). Rubbish birds are poisonous. Nature 360, 19–20.
Rubbish birds are poisonous.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 1:STN:280:DyaK3s%2Fmtlejuw%3D%3D&md5=9428200c6c3a84fac30f3d9adc52259dCAS |

Dumbacher, J., and Fleischer, R. C. (2001). Phylogenetic evidence for color pattern convergence in toxic pitohuis: Mullerian mimicry in birds? Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences 268, 1971–1976.
Phylogenetic evidence for color pattern convergence in toxic pitohuis: Mullerian mimicry in birds?Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 1:CAS:528:DC%2BD3MXotlyltLo%3D&md5=9391d94b1dcc1bd807a789fc25fa56c5CAS |

Dumbacher, J. P., Beehler, B. M., Spande, T. F., Garraffo, H. M., and Daly, J. W. (1992). Homobatrachotoxin in the genus Pitohui: chemical defense in birds? Science 258, 799–801.
Homobatrachotoxin in the genus Pitohui: chemical defense in birds?Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 1:CAS:528:DyaK3sXjs1WltQ%3D%3D&md5=40aae046219972613589dd340ee71381CAS |

Dumbacher, J. P., Spande, T. F., and Daly, J. W. (2000). Batrachotoxin alkaloids from passerine birds: a second toxic bird genus (Ifrita kowaldi) from New Guinea. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 97, 12970–12975.
Batrachotoxin alkaloids from passerine birds: a second toxic bird genus (Ifrita kowaldi) from New Guinea.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 1:CAS:528:DC%2BD3cXosVKqtrY%3D&md5=090b1ce00a67518e6026ef95b1ea1f1bCAS |

Dumbacher, J. P., Deiner, K., Thompson, L., and Fleischer, R. C. (2008). Phylogeny of the avian genus Pitohui and the evolution of toxicity in birds. Molecular Phylogenetics and Evolution 49, 774–781.
Phylogeny of the avian genus Pitohui and the evolution of toxicity in birds.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 1:CAS:528:DC%2BD1cXhsVCntL%2FL&md5=7e85bc99f5cedf0ca49348b8908931d8CAS |

Goodale, E., and Beauchamp, G. (2010). The relationship between leadership and gregariousness in mixed-species bird flocks. Journal of Avian Biology 41, 99–103.
The relationship between leadership and gregariousness in mixed-species bird flocks.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Goodale, E., and Kotagama, S. W. (2005). Testing the roles of species in mixed-species bird flocks of a Sri Lankan rainforest. Journal of Tropical Ecology 21, 669–676.
Testing the roles of species in mixed-species bird flocks of a Sri Lankan rainforest.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Goodale, E., and Kotagama, S. W. (2006a). Context-dependent mimicry by a passerine bird. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences 273, 875–880.
Context-dependent mimicry by a passerine bird.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Goodale, E., and Kotagama, S. W. (2006b). Vocal mimicry by a passerine bird attracts other species involved in mixed-species flocks. Animal Behaviour 72, 471–477.
Vocal mimicry by a passerine bird attracts other species involved in mixed-species flocks.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Kelley, L. A., Coe, R. L., Madden, J. R., and Healy, S. D. (2008). Vocal mimicry in songbirds. Animal Behaviour 76, 521–528.
Vocal mimicry in songbirds.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Kroodsma, D. E., Byers, B. E., Goodale, E., Johnson, S., and Liu, W.-C. (2001). Pseudoreplication in playback experiments, revisited a decade later. Animal Behaviour 61, 1029–1033.
Pseudoreplication in playback experiments, revisited a decade later.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Kunte, K. (2009). The diversity and evolution of Batesian mimicry in Papilio swallowtail butterflies. Evolution 63, 2707–2716.
The diversity and evolution of Batesian mimicry in Papilio swallowtail butterflies.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Landeau, L., and Terborgh, J. (1986). Oddity and the confusion effect in predation. Animal Behaviour 34, 1372–1380.
Oddity and the confusion effect in predation.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Moynihan, M. (1960). Some adaptations which help to promote gregariousness. Proceedings of the International Ornithological Congress 12, 523–541.

Moynihan, M. (1962). The organization and probable evolution of some mixed-species flocks of Neotropical birds. Smithsonian Miscellaneous Collections 143, 1–140.

Moynihan, M. (1968). Social mimicry: character convergence versus character displacement. Evolution 22, 315–331.
Social mimicry: character convergence versus character displacement.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Moynihan, M. (1979). Geographic variation in social behavior and in adaptations to competition among Andean birds. Publications of the Nutall Ornithological Club 18, 1–162.

Müller, F. (1879). Ituna and Thyridia: a remarkable case of mimicry in butterflies. Proceedings of the Entomological Society of London 1879, xx–xxiv.

Munn, C. A., and Terborgh, J. W. (1979). Multi-species territoriality in Neotropical foraging flocks. Condor 81, 338–347.
Multi-species territoriality in Neotropical foraging flocks.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Norman, M. D., Finn, J., and Treganza, T. (2001). Dynamic mimicry in an Indo-Malayan octopus. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences 268, 1755–1758.
Dynamic mimicry in an Indo-Malayan octopus.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 1:STN:280:DC%2BD3MvnvFCrug%3D%3D&md5=fca7fc17a48b56164415d2f0623b4dbaCAS |

Novotny, V., Miller, S. E., Hulcr, J., Drew, R. A. I., Basset, Y., Janda, M., Setliff, G. P., Darrow, K., Stewart, A. J. A., Auga, J., Isua, B., Molem, K., Manumbor, M., Tamtiai, E., Mogia, M., and Weiblen, G. D. (2007). Low beta diversity of herbivorous insects in tropical forests. Nature 448, 692–695.
Low beta diversity of herbivorous insects in tropical forests.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 1:CAS:528:DC%2BD2sXos1yksL0%3D&md5=9fbc51c2aead0027647039b87572e935CAS |

Pfennig, D. W., Harcombe, W. R., and Pfennig, K. S. (2001). Frequency-dependent Batesian mimicry. Nature 410, 323.
Frequency-dependent Batesian mimicry.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 1:CAS:528:DC%2BD3MXitFGktro%3D&md5=b60815527678e9a72ce2472d167c0dbaCAS |

Rowland, H. M., Hoogesteger, T., Ruxton, G. D., Speed, M. P., and Mappes, J. (2010). A tale of 2 signals: signal mimicry between aposematic species enhances predator avoidance learning. Behavioral Ecology 21, 851–860.
A tale of 2 signals: signal mimicry between aposematic species enhances predator avoidance learning.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Ruxton, G. D., Sherratt, T. N., and Speed, M. P. (2004). ‘Avoiding Attack: The Evolutionary Ecology of Crypsis, Warning Signals and Mimicry.’ (Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK.)

Sridhar, H., Beauchamp, G., and Shanker, K. (2009). Why do birds participate in mixed-species foraging flocks? A large-scale synthesis. Animal Behaviour 78, 337–347.
Why do birds participate in mixed-species foraging flocks? A large-scale synthesis.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Wallace, A. L. (1889). ‘Darwinism. An Exposition of the Theory of Natural Selection with Some of its Applications.’ (Macmillan & Co.: London, UK.)

Weibel, A. C., and Moore, W. S. (2005). Plumage convergence in Picoides woodpeckers based on a molecular phylogeny, with emphasis on convergence in Downy and Hairy Woodpeckers. Condor 107, 797–809.

Witt, C. C. (2005). Syntopic breeding suggests mimicry of the Black-and-White Seedeater (Sporophila luctuosa) by the Black-and-White Tanager (Conothraupis speculigera). Ornitologia Neotropical 16, 387–396.