Time allocation to resources by three species of rats (Rattus spp.) in a radial arm maze
Gary W. Witmer A B , Nathan P. Snow A and Rachael S. Moulton AA USDA/APHIS Wildlife Services National Wildlife Research Center, 4101 Laporte Avenue, Fort Collins, CO 80521-2154, USA.
B Corresponding author. Email: gary.w.witmer@usda.gov
Wildlife Research 47(1) 25-33 https://doi.org/10.1071/WR18165
Submitted: 20 October 2018 Accepted: 17 August 2019 Published: 18 December 2019
Abstract
Context: Introduced rats (Rattus spp.) can pose a serious threat to native flora and fauna, especially on islands where most species have evolved in the absence of terrestrial predators. Effective detection and eradication methods for introduced rats are essential to the maintenance of insular ecosystem integrity. Thus, it is important to better understand the behaviour of rats when they first arrive in a new setting.
Aims: To determine whether rats would find some novel stimuli to be significantly more attractive than other novel stimuli.
Methods: An eight-arm radial maze was used to study the behaviour of three species of Rattus finding themselves in a novel environment with various familiar and unfamiliar stimuli.
Key results: Although there were some differences in responses by species and by sex, most rats sought out and spent considerable time in the den box, suggesting an immediate need for security when in an unfamiliar setting. Rats also sought out faeces of conspecifics, suggesting the need for social contact or reproduction. The rats, which had not been food deprived, did not seem interested in food sources, although there was some attraction to the water source.
Implications: The management implications of the present study’s results are two-fold. First, appears that detection of newly arriving rats on islands would be aided by strategic placement of den boxes that are highly acceptable to rats. Managers could then inspect the den boxes periodically (or use a remote sensing system) for evidence of rat presence. Second, the den boxes could be scented with the faeces of other rats to further attract invading rats to the den boxes. This protocol might also hold the rats near the invasion site for a longer period of time before they begin seeking other shelter, food sources or mates. These protocols could give managers increased opportunities to detect any newly invading rats, and potentially increase the available time to deploy a rapid response to the invasion, before the animals begin to widely disperse. Of course, the rats will ultimately seek a source of palatable food, so placing durable, yet palatable, rodenticide bait in the den boxes might further decrease the probability of the invaders establishing a self-sustaining population.
Additional keywords: attractants, invasive species, novel environment.
References
Barnett, S. (1988). Exploring, sampling, neophobia, and feeding. In ‘Rodent Pest Management’. (Ed. I. Prakash.) pp. 295–320. (CRC Press: Boca Raton, FL.)Barnett, S., and Cowan, P. (1976). Activity, exploration, curiosity and fear: an ethological study. Interdisciplinary Science Reviews 1, 43–62.
| Activity, exploration, curiosity and fear: an ethological study.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
Barnett, S., and Spencer, M. (1951). Feeding, social behaviour and interspecific competition in wild rats. Behaviour 3, 229–242.
Barnett, S., Dickson, R., Marples, T., and Radha, E. (1978). Sequences of feeding, sampling, and exploration by wild and laboratory rats. Behavioural Processes 3, 29–43.
| Sequences of feeding, sampling, and exploration by wild and laboratory rats.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 24924375PubMed |
Berdoy, M., and Macdonald, D. (1991). Factors affecting feeding in wild rats. Acta Oecologica 12, 261–279.
Birke, L., and Archer, J. (1983). Some issues and problems in the study of animal exploration. In ‘Exploration in Animals and Humans’. (Eds J. Archer, and L. Birke.) pp. 1–21. (Van Nostrand Reinhold: Berkshire, UK.)
Boice, R. (1971). Laboratizing the wild rat (Rattus norvegicus). Behavior Research Methods and Instrumentation 3, 177–182.
| Laboratizing the wild rat (Rattus norvegicus).Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
Brigham, A., and Sibly, R. (1999). A review of the phenomenon of neophobia. In ‘Advances in Vertebrate Pest Management’. (Eds D. Cowan, and C. Feare.) pp. 67–84. (Filander Verlag: Firth, Germany.)
Brown, K. P. (1997). Predation at nests of two New Zealand endemic passerines; implications for bird community restoration. Pacific Conservation Biology 3, 91–98.
| Predation at nests of two New Zealand endemic passerines; implications for bird community restoration.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
Clapperton, B. (2006). A review of the current knowledge of rodent behavior in relation to control devices. Science for Conservation 263. New Zealand Department of Conservation, Wellington, New Zealand.
Hansen, S., Solter, C., and Jacob, J. (2016). Effect of plant secondary metabolites on feeding behavior of microtine and arvicoline rodent species. Journal of Pest Science 89, 955–963.
| Effect of plant secondary metabolites on feeding behavior of microtine and arvicoline rodent species.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
Hansen, S., Stolter, C., Imholt, C., and Jacob, J. (2017). Like or dislike: responses of rodents to the odor of plant secondary metabolites. Integrative Zoology 12, 428–436.
| Like or dislike: responses of rodents to the odor of plant secondary metabolites.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 27992117PubMed |
Hegab, I., Pan, H., Junyan, D., Wang, A., Yin, B., Yang, S., and Wei, W. (2014). Effects of physical attributes and chemical composition of novel foods on food selection by Norway rats (Rattus norvegicus). Journal of Pest Science 87, 99–106.
| Effects of physical attributes and chemical composition of novel foods on food selection by Norway rats (Rattus norvegicus).Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
Howald, G., Donlan, C., Galvan, J., Russell, J., Parkes, J., Samaniego, A., Wand, Y., Veitch, D., Genovesi, P., Pascal, M., Saunders, A., and Tershy, B. (2007). Invasive rodent eradication on islands. Conservation Biology 21, 1258–1268.
| Invasive rodent eradication on islands.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 17883491PubMed |
Hughes, R. (1997). Intrinsic exploration in animals: motives and measurement. Behavioural Processes 41, 213–226.
| Intrinsic exploration in animals: motives and measurement.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 24896854PubMed |
Ilersich, T., Mazmanian, D., and Roberts, W. (1988). Foraging for covered and uncovered food on a radial maze. Animal Learning & Behavior 16, 388–394.
| Foraging for covered and uncovered food on a radial maze.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
Inglis, J., Shepherd, D., Smith, P., Haynes, P., Bull, D., Cowan, D., and Whitehead, D. (1996). Foraging behavior of wild rats (Rattus norvegicus) towards new food and bait containers. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 47, 175–190.
| Foraging behavior of wild rats (Rattus norvegicus) towards new food and bait containers.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
Jackson, M., Hartley, S., and Linklater, W. (2016). Better food-based baits and lures for invasive rats Rattus spp. and the brushtail possum Trichosurus vulpecula: a bioassay on wild, free-ranging animals. Journal of Pest Science 89, 479–488.
| Better food-based baits and lures for invasive rats Rattus spp. and the brushtail possum Trichosurus vulpecula: a bioassay on wild, free-ranging animals.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
Macdonald, D., Mathews, F., and Berdoy, M. (1999). The behaviour and ecology of Rattus norvegicus: from opportunism to kamikaze tendencies. In ‘Ecologically-Based Management of Rodent Pests’. (Eds G. Singleton, L. Hinds, H. Liers, and Z. Zhang.) Monograph No. 59, pp. 49–80. (Australian Centre for International Agricultural Research (ACIAR): Canberra.)
Mastrangelo, M., Schleich, C., and Zenuto, R. (2009). Short-term effects of an acute exposure to predatory cues on the spatial working and reference memory performance in a subterranean rodent. Animal Behaviour 77, 685–692.
| Short-term effects of an acute exposure to predatory cues on the spatial working and reference memory performance in a subterranean rodent.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
Meehan, A. (Ed.) (1984). ‘Rats and Mice: Their Biology and Control.’ (Rentokil Limited: East Grinstead, UK.)
Mitchell, D. (1976). Experiments in neophobia in wild and laboratory rats: a reevaluation. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 90, 190–197.
| Experiments in neophobia in wild and laboratory rats: a reevaluation.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 1249271PubMed |
Moors, P. J., and Atkinson, I. A. E. (1984). Predation on seabirds by introduced animals, and factors affecting its severity. In ‘Status and Conservation of the World’s Seabirds’. ICBP Technical Publication Number 2. (Eds J. P. Croxall, P. G. H. Evans, and R. W. Schrieber.) pp. 667–690. (International Council for Bird Preservation (ICBP): Cambridge, UK.)
Priyambodo, S., and Pelz, H. (2003). Studies on neophobic behavior in Norway rats from farms in Germany. In ‘Rats, Mice and People: Rodent Biology and Management’. Monograph No. 96. (Eds G. Singleton, L. Hinds, C. Krebs, and D. Spratt.) pp. 155–158. (Australian Centre for International Agricultural Research (ACIAR): Canberra.)
Puth, L., and Post, D. (2005). Studying invasion: have we missed the boat? Ecology Letters 8, 715–721.
| Studying invasion: have we missed the boat?Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
Russell, P. (1983). Psychological studies of exploration in animals: a reappraisal. In ‘Exploration in Animals and Humans’. (Eds J. Archer, and L. Birke.) pp. 22–54. (Van Nostrand Reinhold: Berkshire, UK.)
Russell, J. C. (2007). Invasion ecology and genetics of Norway rats on New Zealand islands. Ph.D. Thesis, University of Auckland, Auckland.
Russell, J. C., Towns, D. R., Anderson, S. H., and Clout, M. N. (2005). Intercepting the first rat ashore. Nature 437, 1107.
| Intercepting the first rat ashore.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 16237434PubMed |
Russell, J., Beaven, B., MacKay, J., Towns, D., and Clout, M. (2008). Testing island biosecurity systems for invasive rats. Wildlife Research 35, 215–221.
| Testing island biosecurity systems for invasive rats.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
Shepherd, D., and Inglis, I. (1987). Feeding behavior, social interactions and poison bait consumption by a family group of wild rats living in semi-natural conditions. In ‘Stored Products Pest’. Monograph No. 37. (Ed. T. Lawson.) pp. 97–105. (British Crop Protection Council: Croydon, UK.)
Spurr, E. B., O’Connor, C. E., Morriss, G. A., and Turner, J. (2006). Bait station preferences of Norway rats. DOC Research & Development Series 255. Department of Conservation, Wellington, New Zealand.
Spurr, E. B., Morriss, G. A., Turner, J., O’Connor, C. E., and Fisher, P. (2007). Bait station preferences of ship rats. DOC Research & Development Series 271. Department of Conservation, Wellington, New Zealand.
Takács, S., Musso, A., Gries, R., Rozenberg, E., Borden, J., Brodie, B., and Gries, G. (2018). New food baits for trapping house mice, black rats and brown rats. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 200, 130–135.
| New food baits for trapping house mice, black rats and brown rats.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
Timm, R., and Salmon, T. (1988). Behavior. In ‘Rodent Pest Management’. (Ed. I. Prakash.) pp. 225–235. (CRC Press: Boca Raton, FL.)
Towns, D., and Broome, K. (2003). From small Maria to massive Campbell: forty years of rat eradications from New Zealand islands. New Zealand Journal of Zoology 30, 377–398.
| From small Maria to massive Campbell: forty years of rat eradications from New Zealand islands.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
Van Haaren, F., Wouters, M., and Van de Poll, N. (1987). Absence of behavioral differences between male and female rats in different radial-maze procedures. Physiology & Behavior 39, 409–412.
| Absence of behavioral differences between male and female rats in different radial-maze procedures.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
Wallace, R. (2003). Patterns of feeding and food pellet retrieval by Norway rats during food deprivation. Journal of Ethology 21, 111–116.
Witmer, G., and Shiels, A. (2018). Ecology, impacts, and management of invasive rodents in the United States. In ‘Ecology and Management of Terrestrial Vertebrate Invasive Species in the United States’. (Eds W. Pitt, J. Beasley, and G. Witmer.) pp. 385–388. (CRC Press: Boca Raton, FL.)
Witmer, G., Burke, P., Jojola, S., and Dunlevy, P. (2006). The biology of introduced Norway rats on Kiska Island, Alaska, and an evaluation of an eradication approach. Northwest Science 80, 191–198.
Witmer, G., Burke, P., and Jojola, S. (2008). An evaluation of the effectiveness of potential Norway rat attractants. In ‘Proceedings of the 23rd Vertebrate Pest Conference’, 17–20 March 2008, San Diego, CA, USA. (Eds R. Timm, and M. Madon.) pp. 35–38. (University of California: Davis, CA.)
Witmer, G., Snow, N., and Burke, P. (2010). Potential attractants for detecting and removing invading Gambian giant pouched rats (Cricetomys gambianus). Pest Management Science 66, 412–416.
| 20013871PubMed |
Witmer, G., Snow, N., and Moulton, R. (2014). Responses by wild house mice (Mus musculus) to various stimuli in a novel environment. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 159, 99–106.
| Responses by wild house mice (Mus musculus) to various stimuli in a novel environment.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
Ylonen, H., and Brown, J. (2007). Fear and the foraging, breeding, and sociality of rodents. In ‘Rodent Societies: an Ecological and Evolutionary Perspective’. (Eds J. Wolff, and P. Sherman.) pp. 328–341. (University of Chicago Press: Chicago, IL.)