Register      Login
Wildlife Research Wildlife Research Society
Ecology, management and conservation in natural and modified habitats
RESEARCH ARTICLE

Effective field-based methods to quantify personality in brushtail possums (Trichosurus vulpecula)

Valentina S. A. Mella A C , Jordan Krucler A , Lakshmi Sunderasan A , Jason Hawkins A , Anushika P. H. M. Herath A , Kyla C. Johnstone A , Sandra M. Troxell-Smith A B , Peter B. Banks A and Clare McArthur A
+ Author Affiliations
- Author Affiliations

A School of Life and Environmental Sciences, Heydon Laurence Building A08, The University of Sydney, Camperdown, NSW 2006, Australia.

B Department of Biological Sciences, University of Illinois at Chicago, IL, USA.

C Corresponding author. Email: valentina.mella@sydney.edu.au

Wildlife Research 43(4) 332-340 https://doi.org/10.1071/WR15216
Submitted: 20 November 2015  Accepted: 15 May 2016   Published: 4 July 2016

Abstract

Context: There is growing recognition of the importance of animal personality in wildlife ecology and management. Individuals that differ consistently in their behaviours from others of the same population are considered to exhibit different personalities. Personality can be easily quantified with repeat tests on animals held in captivity. However, captive-based tests may not always be possible for logistical reasons and tests conducted in captivity can alter naturally occurring behaviours. Development of methods to assess personality in the field over short-term capture is an important alternative to long-term captive tests.

Aims: To develop and refine field-based tests that can be used to easily define personality traits of wild common brushtail possum (Trichosurus vulpecula), eliminating the need to bring individuals into captivity for an extended period of time.

Methods: We developed a series of short-term capture protocols to quantify personality traits of the common brushtail possum in the field, using handling-bag tests, modified open-field tests, trap-behaviour tests and release tests.

Key results: Personality traits of brushtail possums could be measured in several different ways, but refining methods was crucial to reveal traits efficiently. Behaviours related to several personality traits (boldness, docility and activity/exploration) could be measured rapidly in the field with our methods, and were repeatable over time.

Conclusions: Rapid, reliable and easy-to-perform quantification of personality of brushtail possums in the field is possible. This may sometimes be the only available option, and has advantages over long captive trials in terms of animal welfare and cost.

Implications: The present study provides a baseline for future research on the personality of common brushtail possums and its application to management and control strategies for the species.

Additional keywords: activity, boldness, docility, exploration.


References

Bell, A. M. (2007). Future directions in behavioural syndromes research. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences 274, 755–761.
Future directions in behavioural syndromes research.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Bell, A. M., Hankison, S., and Laskowski, K. (2009). The repeatability of behaviour: a meta-analysis. Animal Behaviour 77, 771–783.
The repeatability of behaviour: a meta-analysis.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 24707058PubMed |

Biro, P. (2012). Do rapid assays predict repeatability in labile (behavioural) traits? Animal Behaviour 83, 1295–1300.
Do rapid assays predict repeatability in labile (behavioural) traits?Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Biro, P. A. (2013). Are most samples of animals systematically biased? Consistent individual trait differences bias samples despite random sampling. Oecologia 171, 339–345.
Are most samples of animals systematically biased? Consistent individual trait differences bias samples despite random sampling.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 22885993PubMed |

Blumstein, D. T., and Daniel, J. C. (2007). ‘Quantifying Behaviour the JWatcher Way.’ (Sinauer Associates: Sunderland, MA.)

Boyer, N., Réale, D., Marmet, J., Pisanu, B., and Chapuis, J. (2010). Personality, space use and tick load in an introduced population of Siberian chipmunks Tamias sibiricus. Journal of Animal Ecology 79, 538–547.
Personality, space use and tick load in an introduced population of Siberian chipmunks Tamias sibiricus.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 20202009PubMed |

Burnham, K. P., and Anderson, D. R. (2002). ‘Model Selection and Multimodel Inference: a Practical Information-theoretic Approach.’ (Springer Verlag, New York.)

Butler, S. J., Whittingham, M. J., Quinn, J. L., and Cresswell, W. (2006). Time in captivity, individual differences and foraging behaviour in wild-caught chaffinches. Behaviour 143, 535–548.
Time in captivity, individual differences and foraging behaviour in wild-caught chaffinches.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Careau, V., Thomas, D., Humphries, M. M., and Réale, D. (2008). Energy metabolism and animal personality. Oikos 117, 641–653.
Energy metabolism and animal personality.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Carter, A. J., Marshall, H. H., Heinsohn, R., and Cowlishaw, G. (2012a). How not to measure boldness: novel object and antipredator responses are not the same in wild baboons. Animal Behaviour 84, 603–609.
How not to measure boldness: novel object and antipredator responses are not the same in wild baboons.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Carter, A. J., Heinsohn, R., Goldizen, A. W., and Biro, P. A. (2012b). Boldness, trappability and sampling bias in wild lizards. Animal Behaviour 83, 1051–1058.
Boldness, trappability and sampling bias in wild lizards.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Clinchy, M., Krebs, C. J., and Jarman, P. J. (2001). Dispersal sinks and handling effects: interpreting the role of immigration in common brushtail possum populations. Journal of Animal Ecology 70, 515–526.
Dispersal sinks and handling effects: interpreting the role of immigration in common brushtail possum populations.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Clout, M. N., and Ericksen, K. (2000). Anatomy of a disastrous success: the brushtail possum as an invasive species. In ‘The Brushtail Possum: Biology, Impact and Management of an Introduced Marsupial’. (Ed. T. L. Montague.) pp. 1–9. (Manaaki Whenua Press: Lincoln, New Zealand.)

Dammhahn, M. (2012). Are personality differences in a small iteroparous mammal maintained by a life-history trade-off? Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences 279, 2645–2651.
Are personality differences in a small iteroparous mammal maintained by a life-history trade-off?Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Dammhahn, M., and Almeling, L. (2012). Is risk taking during foraging a personality trait? A field test for cross-context consistency in boldness. Animal Behaviour 84, 1131–1139.
Is risk taking during foraging a personality trait? A field test for cross-context consistency in boldness.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Dingemanse, N. J., and Dochtermann, N. A. (2013). Quantifying individual variation in behaviour: mixed-effect modelling approaches. Journal of Animal Ecology 82, 39–54.
Quantifying individual variation in behaviour: mixed-effect modelling approaches.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 23171297PubMed |

Dingemanse, N. J., Both, C., Drent, P. J., van Oers, K., and van Noordwijk, A. J. (2002). Repeatability and heritability of exploratory behaviour in great tits from the wild. Animal Behaviour 64, 929–938.
Repeatability and heritability of exploratory behaviour in great tits from the wild.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Dingemanse, N. J., Bouwman, K. M., van de Pol, M., van Overveld, T., Patrick, S. C., Matthysen, E., and Quinn, J. L. (2012). Variation in personality and behavioural plasticity across four populations of the great tit Parus major. Journal of Animal Ecology 81, 116–126.
Variation in personality and behavioural plasticity across four populations of the great tit Parus major.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 21692798PubMed |

Edwards, H. A., Winney, I. S., Schroeder, J., and Dugdale, H. L. (2013). Do rapid assays predict repeatability in labile (behavioural) traits? A reply to Biro. Animal Behaviour 85, e1–e3.
Do rapid assays predict repeatability in labile (behavioural) traits? A reply to Biro.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Efford, M., Warburton, B., and Spencer, N. (2000). Home-range changes by brushtail possums in response to control. Wildlife Research 27, 117–127.
Home-range changes by brushtail possums in response to control.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Eymann, J., Herbert, C. A., and Cooper, D. W. (2006). Management issues of urban common brushtail possums Trichosurus vulpecula: a loved or hated neighbour. Australian Mammalogy 28, 153–171.
Management issues of urban common brushtail possums Trichosurus vulpecula: a loved or hated neighbour.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Groothuis, T. G. G., and Maestripieri, D. (2013). Parental influences on offspring personality traits in oviparous and palcental vertebrates. In ‘Animal Personalities: Behaviour, Physiology and Evolution’. (Eds C. Carere and D. Maestripieri.) pp. 317–352. (The University of Chicago Press: Chicago, IL.)

Herborn, K. A., Macleod, R., Miles, W. T. S., Schofield, A. N. B., Alexander, L., and Arnold, K. E. (2010). Personality in captivity reflects personality in the wild. Animal Behaviour 79, 835–843.
Personality in captivity reflects personality in the wild.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Kerle, J. A., and How, R. A. (2008). Common brushtail possum. In ‘The Mammals of Australia’. (Eds S. Van Dyck and R. Strahan.) pp. 274–276. (Reed New Holland: Sydney.)

Kluen, E., Kuhn, S., Kempenaers, B., and Brommer, J. E. (2012). A simple cage test captures intrinsic differences in aspects of personality across individuals in a passerine bird. Animal Behaviour 84, 279–287.
A simple cage test captures intrinsic differences in aspects of personality across individuals in a passerine bird.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Koolhaas, J. M., Korte, S. M., De Boer, S. F., Van Der Vegt, B. J., Van Reenen, C. G., Hopster, H., De Jong, I. C., Ruis, M. A. W., and Blokhuis, H. J. (1999). Coping styles in animals: current status in behavior and stress-physiology. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 23, 925–935.
Coping styles in animals: current status in behavior and stress-physiology.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 1:STN:280:DC%2BD3c%2Fks1amsA%3D%3D&md5=360ff83232701973092a2608c6807197CAS | 10580307PubMed |

Kralj-Fišer, S., and Schneider, J. M. (2012). Individual behavioural consistency and plasticity in an urban spider. Animal Behaviour 84, 197–204.
Individual behavioural consistency and plasticity in an urban spider.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Krucler, J. (2013). Quantifying ecologically relevant personality traits and their link to movement patterns in wild common brushtail possums (Trichosurus vulpecula). Honours thesis, University of Sydney, Sydney.

Kurvers, R. H. J. M., Eijkelenkamp, B., van Oers, K., van Lith, B., van Wieren, S. E., Ydenberg, R. C., and Prins, H. H. T. (2009). Personality differences explain leadership in barnacle geese. Animal Behaviour 78, 447–453.
Personality differences explain leadership in barnacle geese.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Martin, J. G. A., and Réale, D. (2008a). Animal temperament and human disturbance: implications for the response of wildlife to tourism. Behavioural Processes 77, 66–72.
Animal temperament and human disturbance: implications for the response of wildlife to tourism.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 1:STN:280:DC%2BD1c7jslyhtQ%3D%3D&md5=2d5b5799144f15cfb19c5fb156c13323CAS |

Martin, J. G. A., and Réale, D. (2008b). Temperament, risk assessment and habituation to novelty in eastern chipmunks, Tamias striatus. Animal Behaviour 75, 309–318.
Temperament, risk assessment and habituation to novelty in eastern chipmunks, Tamias striatus.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Martin, J. G. A., Nussey, D. H., Wilson, A. J., and Réale, D. (2011). Measuring individual differences in reaction norms in field and experimental studies: a power analysis of random regression models. Methods in Ecology and Evolution 2, 362–374.
Measuring individual differences in reaction norms in field and experimental studies: a power analysis of random regression models.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

McDonald-Madden, E., Akers, L. K., Brenner, D. J., Howell, S., Patullo, B. W., and Elgar, M. A. (2000). Possums in the park: efficient foraging under the risk of predation or of competition? Australian Journal of Zoology 48, 155–160.
Possums in the park: efficient foraging under the risk of predation or of competition?Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Mella, V. S. A., Banks, P. B., and McArthur, C. (2014). Negotiating multiple cues of predation risk in a landscape of fear: what scares free-ranging brushtail possums? Journal of Zoology 294, 22–30.
Negotiating multiple cues of predation risk in a landscape of fear: what scares free-ranging brushtail possums?Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Mella, V. S. A., Ward, A. J. W., Banks, P. B., and Mcarthur, C. (2015). Personality affects the foraging response of a mammalian herbivore to the dual costs of food and fear. Oecologia 177, 293–303.
Personality affects the foraging response of a mammalian herbivore to the dual costs of food and fear.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Michelena, P., Sibbald, A. M., Erhard, H. W., and McLeod, J. E. (2009). Effects of group size and personality on social foraging: the distribution of sheep across patches. Behavioral Ecology 20, 145–152.
Effects of group size and personality on social foraging: the distribution of sheep across patches.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Minderman, J., Reid, J. M., Evans, P. G. H., and Whittingham, M. J. (2009). Personality traits in wild starlings: exploration behavior and environmental sensitivity. Behavioral Ecology 20, 830–837.
Personality traits in wild starlings: exploration behavior and environmental sensitivity.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Montiglio, P., Garant, D., Thomas, D., and Réale, D. (2010). Individual variation in temporal activity patterns in open-field tests. Animal Behaviour 80, 905–912.
Individual variation in temporal activity patterns in open-field tests.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Nathan, R., Getz, W. M., Revilla, E., Holyoak, M., Kadmon, R., Saltz, D., and Smouse, P. E. (2008). A movement ecology paradigm for unifying organismal movement research. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA 105, 19052–19059.
A movement ecology paradigm for unifying organismal movement research.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 1:CAS:528:DC%2BD1cXhsFamurfP&md5=1f696f90d65a622796358e791164c207CAS |

Nugent, G., Whitford, J., Sweetapple, P., Duncan, R., and Holland, P. (2010). Effect of one-hit control on the density of possums (Trichosurus vulpecula) and their impacts on native forest. In ‘Science for Conservation’. (Department of Conservation: Wellington, New Zealand.)

Petelle, M. B., McCoy, D. E., Alejandro, V., Martin, J. G. A., and Blumstein, D. T. (2013). Development of boldness and docility in yellow-bellied marmots. Animal Behaviour 86, 1147–1154.
Development of boldness and docility in yellow-bellied marmots.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Réale, D., Reader, S. M., Sol, D., McDougall, P. T., and Dingemanse, N. J. (2007). Integrating animal temperament within ecology and evolution. Biological Reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society 82, 291–318.
Integrating animal temperament within ecology and evolution.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 17437562PubMed |

Rekilä, T., Harri, M., and Ahola, L. (1997). Validation of the feeding test as an index of fear in farmed blue (Alopex lagopus) and silver foxes (Vulpes vulpes). Physiology & Behavior 62, 805–810.
Validation of the feeding test as an index of fear in farmed blue (Alopex lagopus) and silver foxes (Vulpes vulpes).Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Rodríguez-Prieto, I., Martín, J., and Fernández-Juricic, E. (2011). Individual variation in behavioural plasticity: direct and indirect effects of boldness, exploration and sociability on habituation to predators in lizards. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences 278, 266–273.
Individual variation in behavioural plasticity: direct and indirect effects of boldness, exploration and sociability on habituation to predators in lizards.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Schütz, K. E., Ågren, E., Amundin, M., Röken, B., Palme, R., and Mörner, T. (2006). Behavioral and physiological responses of trap-induced stress in European badgers. The Journal of Wildlife Management 70, 884–891.
Behavioral and physiological responses of trap-induced stress in European badgers.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Sinn, D. L., Apiolaza, L. A., and Moltschaniwskyj, N. A. (2006). Heritability and fitness-related consequences of squid personality traits. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 19, 1437–1447.
Heritability and fitness-related consequences of squid personality traits.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 1:STN:280:DC%2BD28vosl2gsw%3D%3D&md5=312c516cfac3e715f0d3a1a87f996a83CAS | 16910975PubMed |

Stamps, J. A., and Groothuis, T. G. G. (2010). The development of animal personality: relevance, concepts and perspectives. Biological Reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society 85, 301–325.
The development of animal personality: relevance, concepts and perspectives.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Statham, M., and Statham, H. L. (1997). Movements and habits of brushtail possums (Trichosurus vulpecula Kerr) in an urban area. Wildlife Research 24, 715–726.
Movements and habits of brushtail possums (Trichosurus vulpecula Kerr) in an urban area.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Uher, J. (2011). Individual behavioral phenotypes: an integrative meta-theoretical framework. Why ‘behavioral syndromes’ are not analogs of ‘personality’. Developmental Psychobiology 53, 521–548.
Individual behavioral phenotypes: an integrative meta-theoretical framework. Why ‘behavioral syndromes’ are not analogs of ‘personality’.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 21432848PubMed |

van Oers, K., and Sinn, D. L. (2013). Quantitative and Molecular Genetics of Personality. In ‘Animal Personalities: Behaviour, Physiology and Evolution’. (Eds C. Carere and D. Maestripieri.) pp. 149–200. (The University of Chicago Press: Chicago, IL.)

Walsh, R. N., and Cummins, R. A. (1976). The open-field test: a critical review. Psychological Bulletin 83, 482–504.
The open-field test: a critical review.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 1:STN:280:DC%2BD2szmvVCrsg%3D%3D&md5=bfc91d99d28a9a36760b8d14c355fb26CAS | 17582919PubMed |

Ward, A. J. W., Thomas, P., Hart, P. J. B., and Krause, J. (2004). Correlates of boldness in three-spined sticklebacks (Gasterosteus aculeatus). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 55, 561–568.
Correlates of boldness in three-spined sticklebacks (Gasterosteus aculeatus).Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Wilson, A. D. M., and Krause, J. (2012). Personality and metamorphosis: is behavioral variation consistent across ontogenetic niche shifts? Behavioral Ecology 23, 1316–1323.
Personality and metamorphosis: is behavioral variation consistent across ontogenetic niche shifts?Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Wilson, A. D. M., and McLaughlin, R. L. (2007). Behavioural syndromes in brook charr, Salvelinus fontinalis: prey-search in the field corresponds with space use in novel laboratory situations. Animal Behaviour 74, 689–698.
Behavioural syndromes in brook charr, Salvelinus fontinalis: prey-search in the field corresponds with space use in novel laboratory situations.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Wilson, D. S., Coleman, K., Clarke, A. B., and Biederman, L. (1993). Shy–bold continuum in pumpkinseed sunfish (Lepomis gibbosus): an ecological study of a psychological trait. Journal of Comparative Psychology 107, 250–260.
Shy–bold continuum in pumpkinseed sunfish (Lepomis gibbosus): an ecological study of a psychological trait.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Wilson, D. S., Clark, A. B., Coleman, K., and Dearstyne, T. (1994). Shyness and boldness in humans and other animals. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 9, 442–446.
Shyness and boldness in humans and other animals.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Wilson, A. D. M., Godin, J. J., and Ward, A. J. W. (2010). Boldness and reproductive fitness correlates in the eastern mosquitofish, Gambusia holbrooki. Ethology 116, 96–104.
Boldness and reproductive fitness correlates in the eastern mosquitofish, Gambusia holbrooki.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |