Register      Login
Wildlife Research Wildlife Research Society
Ecology, management and conservation in natural and modified habitats
RESEARCH ARTICLE (Open Access)

Factors influencing the success of hand-reared juvenile brushtail possums (Trichosurus vulpecula) released into the wild

Valentina S. A. Mella https://orcid.org/0000-0001-5577-2487 A B * , Callum Gillies B C , Clare McArthur https://orcid.org/0000-0002-7867-414X B , Elliot Webb https://orcid.org/0000-0003-4761-1493 B and Catherine Herbert B
+ Author Affiliations
- Author Affiliations

A Sydney School of Veterinary Science, The University of Sydney, Sydney, NSW 2006, Australia.

B School of Life and Environmental Sciences, The University of Sydney, Sydney, NSW 2006, Australia.

C Victoria Department of Environment, Land, Water and Planning, Water and Catchments, Melbourne, Vic., Australia.

* Correspondence to: valentina.mella@sydney.edu.au

Handling Editor: Jonathan Webb

Wildlife Research 50(2) 96-107 https://doi.org/10.1071/WR22056
Submitted: 25 March 2022  Accepted: 26 May 2022   Published: 26 July 2022

© 2023 The Author(s) (or their employer(s)). Published by CSIRO Publishing. This is an open access article distributed under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License (CC BY-NC-ND)

Abstract

Context: Wildlife is ubiquitous in urban environments, resulting in frequent interactions with humans and human infrastructure. The result of these interactions is often negative, in the form of road injury, orphaning of dependent young or eviction from natural home ranges. Wildlife rehabilitation programmes are devised to counter these negative interactions. However, the success of current management strategies is rarely assessed.

Aims: We aimed to determine whether short-term survival of juvenile hand-reared common brushtail possums (Trichosurus vulpecula) released in the wild was a function of (1) intrinsic factors such as sex, individual personality and level of human habituation, and (2) extrinsic factors such as release practice (soft vs hard-release) or location (urban vs rural). We also evaluated the relationship between habituation to humans and time spent in care by possums, if presence of conspecifics during the rehabilitation process influenced the development of individual personality, and if this differed in hand-raised animals compared with wild individuals.

Methods: We radio-tracked and monitored 20 hand-reared juvenile possums (10 females and 10 males) for up to 40 days after release in the wild.

Key results: Eight possums (40%) survived until the end of the study, nine possums (45%) were killed by foxes or had to be returned into care and three possums (15%) had unknown fates (i.e. lost VHF signal). We found that more exploratory individuals and those less human-habituated were more likely to be successful in the wild in both rural and urban areas, whether or not they were hard or soft released.

Conclusions: Our results suggest that personality is a key criterion to consider when evaluating the success of rehabilitation programmes. Behavioural traits of hand-reared brushtail possums differed significantly from those of wild individuals, showing that captive conditions can affect the development of personality. Hand-reared possums that spent more time in care were also more likely to display highly human-habituated behaviours.

Implications: By demonstrating which factors influence success of hand-reared wildlife after release in the wild, our results help in evaluating current rearing and release practices, and improve the evidence base for developing best practice wildlife rehabilitation guidelines.

Keywords: exploration, hand-rearing, human habituation, marsupial, personality, post-release monitoring, survival, wildlife rehabilitation.

Introduction

Anthropogenic activities such as urbanisation cause major changes to the natural environment (Tarsitano 2006), including habitat destruction and fragmentation (Marzluff and Ewing 2001; Pekin and Pijanowski 2012). These changes often increase the number of negative animal–human interactions, resulting in injured, orphaned or displaced wildlife (Russell et al. 2009). Rehabilitation programmes are established to mitigate these impacts and foster animals to a healthy state to release them back into the wild (Guy et al. 2013). Hand-rearing, the process of feeding and caring for a young animal (e.g. orphaned or injured) until it is independent and ready for release (Tribe and Brown 2000), is often required. Rehabilitation programmes have three main aims: (1) animal survival post-release; (2) settlement or dispersal from release site; and (3) successful reproduction (Teixeira et al. 2007).

An integral component of rehabilitation programmes is the release stage, where animals are released from captivity into the wild. However, many rehabilitation programmes fail to monitor the post-release survival of animals, or to assess the factors influencing survival post-release. Monitoring of individuals after release in the wild is necessary to assess the success of rehabilitation programmes (Fischer and Lindenmayer 2000) and whether they are meeting the three rehabilitation aims.

Many factors can affect survival in the wild (Cope et al. 2022a). These factors can be both intrinsic (i.e. due to the characteristics of the individual, e.g. health status, body mass, sex and personality) or extrinsic (i.e. due to the characteristic of the release environment, e.g. quality of habitat or type of release method used) (Ario et al. 2018). For example, larger male little penguins (Eudyptula minor) had significantly higher survival rates than smaller females after release into the wild (Goldsworthy et al. 2000). The effect of personality on post-release survival of rehabilitated animals is species dependent (Bremner-Harrison et al. 2004; Rasmussen et al. 2021), with bold captive-raised swift foxes (Vulpes velox) dying quickly after release (Bremner-Harrison et al. 2004), but bold hand-reared Tasmanian devils (Sarcophilus harrisii) surviving well (Sinn et al. 2014).

Animals held in captivity can be stressed (Cope et al. 2022b) undergo behavioural changes compared with their wild counterparts, and may become habituated, tame or dependent on humans (McPhee and Carlstead 2010; Tolhurst et al. 2016). Human habituation of captive or hand-reared animals (McDougall et al. 2006) has been shown to increase with time in care (McPhee 2004), with animals displaying decreased responsiveness to humans. Negative behaviours associated with human-habituated wildlife include seeking refuge less, limited predator recognition and less reactivity to loud noises (McPhee and Carlstead 2010). These behaviours are seen as potentially detrimental to release success, and often result in predation of highly human-habituated individuals after release in the wild (Van Heezik et al. 1999; Rödl et al. 2007).

The release method employed in rehabilitation programmes can also affect survival. The two common methods used for releasing individuals in the wild are soft (delayed) and hard (immediate) release (Parker et al. 2012). Soft release involves a period of acclimatisation to the new environment, where animals are maintained in semicaptive conditions and food and shelter are provided in an enclosed safe area within the release site before final release (Campbell and Croft 2001). Hard-release methods do not include the provision of food and shelter, and rehabilitated animals are immediately released in the wild without any further support (Campbell and Croft 2001). The effectiveness of each method can vary with the species being released, and should therefore be explored in rehabilitation programmes. For instance, swift foxes survive longer if soft release is used (Sasmal et al. 2015), but for stitchbirds (Notiomystis cincta), hard release is more effective (Richardson et al. 2015).

Although the survival of animals post-release may be affected by a number of intrinsic and extrinsic factors, most studies only consider/monitor one or two factors (Bannister et al. 2020). Our study aimed to: (1) determine both intrinsic (i.e. sex, individual personality, level of human habituation) and extrinsic (i.e. release method, release environment) factors affecting survival of hand-reared juvenile brushtail possums (Trichosurus vulpecula), during the first 40 days after release, when animals are most vulnerable (Hamilton et al. 2010; Bannister et al. 2020); and (2) provide improved management recommendations based on our results to help inform wildlife rehabilitation organisations and future release programmes. We also explored factors that can affect the development of individual characteristics, such as how time in care, presence of conspecifics during rehabilitation and captivity can influence the development of personality and habituation to humans. Findings of the study highlight potential downfalls and opportunities to ameliorate rehabilitation management processes.


Materials and methods

Study species

Brushtail possums (hereafter possums) are Australian nocturnal arboreal marsupials, well adapted to urban landscapes (Eymann et al. 2006). They have one of the widest distributions of any Australian marsupial (Helgen and Jackson 2015), but populations have significantly declined in the last two centuries, with severe reduction in density and distribution range (Kerle et al. 1992; Kerle 2001). Despite their relatively high presentation rates in wildlife rehabilitation programmes (WIRES 2019), there is limited understanding of the factors affecting the fate of individuals after rehabilitation and release in the wild (but see May et al. 2016).

Study design

Pre-release stage

Possums were sourced in collaboration with a volunteer wildlife care organisation (Wildlife Information, Rescue and Education Service Incorporated; WIRES) in New South Wales, Australia. Animals remained with their volunteer wildllife rehabilitator until the release stage.

The pre-release stage of the study involved collecting information on intrinsic (i.e. body mass at rescue and release, sex, individual personality and level of human habituation) and extrinsic factors (i.e. release method used and release environment) that may affect survival of individual possums. We also collected other life-history information for each possum such as their date of rescue (used here to calculate the total days spent in care) and whether they were raised with any conspecific.

Behavioural tests

We quantified personality traits by employing an open-field test, specifically designed for possums (see Mella et al. 2016). Possums were placed in an open-field apparatus, consisting of a vertical structure with four levels and adjoining holes to allow movement (Fig. 1). We placed a container with food in each level to encourage feeding in a novel environment [a measure of boldness in Mella et al. (2016)]. Possums were initially placed into the lowest level of the arena, and were then filmed from a distance for 5 min using a video camera (JVC GZ-EX555BSD HD camcorder). Behaviours relative to exploration and boldness were later quantified using the programme J-Watcher (Blumstein and Daniel 2007). All tests were performed at night (between 2000 and 2400 hours) when possums are active (Helgen and Jackson 2015). Each test was performed twice (with at least 1 week between tests), at a maximum 7 days before possums were released in the wild.


Fig. 1.  A brushtail possum in the open-field test apparatus.
Click to zoom


Human habituation

Many possums in the study exhibited behaviours that would be considered atypical for wild possums and indicative of some level of human habituation. These behaviours included climbing and resting on the handler when returned to their enclosures. Because these behaviours might affect survival in the wild (Elsbeth McPhee 2004), we quantified each possum’s level of human habituation (i.e. low, medium and high) by scoring behaviours in response to the presence of humans during capture and handling (Table 1).


Table 1.  Levels of human habituation for brushtail possums with corresponding behaviours in response to capture and handling.
T1


Body mass, microchipping and collaring

Before being released, each possum was weighed to determine release mass (5 kg; Pesola, Switzerland), and uniquely marked using a microchip (Bio-mark ISO FDX-B) for future identification. At least 3 days before release (to allow acclimation), we equipped possums with VHF collars (UG V5 VHF collar; Sirtrack, New Zealand, <3% body weight; Fig. 2c), fitted with mortality signal and a cotton thread weak link [as in Cawthen and Munks (2011)]. The weak link ensured each collar would come off with time as the thread weakened with weathering. This was necessary because all possums in the study were juveniles and still growing at the time of release. The weak link is designed to last a maximum of 45 days and is recommended for short-term monitoring studies of juvenile individuals (Cawthen and Munks 2011).


Fig. 2.  (a) aviary and fresh food supplemented as part of the soft release, (b) a nest box in a tree used as part of the hard release, and (c) a possum pair fitted with VHF collars before release in the wild.
Click to zoom


Post-release stage

Possums were released in groups of three to six individuals, depending on the number WIRES defined as ready for release at that time. Becasue the possums came into care at different times and stages of development, they were not all ready for release at the same time. For this reason, we had different release dates for each group of possums. WIRES employs a ‘buddy system’, where young possums that come into care are paired with other joeys of similar body mass, with the aim to reduce stress and dependence on the human foster wildlife rehabilitator (WIRES 2017). All individuals used in the study had at least one ‘buddy’ they grew up with and with which they were released.

Release sites

The NSW Government guidelines for release of rehabilitated fauna (OEH 2011) suggest that large numbers of individuals should not be released at a single location to avoid saturating the environment. Therefore, we had five different release sites and each group of possums was randomly allocated to a release location. Release sites were selected by WIRES but possum buddies were randomly allocated a release location. These included both urban (i.e. areas surrounding residential or metropolitan infrastructures with limited natural corridors) and rural sites (i.e. less suburban with a high tree cover percentage and backing onto a conservation area or national park).


Release methods

Possum buddies were randomly allocated a release treatment (i.e. soft or hard release), then transported to their release site and released at dusk (1800–2000 hours). Soft-release possums were kept for a week in a portable aviary (3 m H × 2 m L × 2 m D) under dense non-isolated trees at the release site, with food supplementation (native foliage) to allow acclimatisation to local conditions (Fig. 2a). In the second week, the aviary door was opened and food supplementation was gradually reduced, to encourage the possums to explore beyond the familiar aviary. In the third week, the aviary was removed from the release site. All possums had vacated the aviary before its removal.

Hard-release possums were placed in the environment in nest boxes, which were left at the site at the time of the release, with no other provisions or support. Nest boxes were placed at least 200 m away from the soft-release aviary, about 5 m high in non-isolated native trees (Fig. 2b). We released hard-release possums in the wild at the same time as soft-release possums (i.e. when the soft-release aviary door was opened in week 2).


Survival

Following release, possums were tracked using radio-telemetry until collar loss or mortality (maximum 40 days). Because our study used short-term monitoring and juvenile possums, we could not use dispersal and reproduction as proxies for success. Therefore, we used short-term survival as indication of successful release. If a possum was alive at the end of the 40-day monitoring period or until its collar came off, it was recorded as a successful release (survival = yes). If a possum died or we had to take it back into care after release (because it became injured or sick), we recorded it as a failed release (survival = no). Animal remains found during post-release monitoring were assessed for cause of death following Augee et al. (1996).



Results

We released 20 hand-reared juvenile possums (10 females and 10 males) within the Sydney metropolitan area: seven possums at urban release sites (four males and three females) and 13 (six males and seven females) at rural release sites. We used a soft-release method for nine individuals (five males and four females) and a hard-release method for the remaining 11 (five males and six females). We used both methods at each urban and rural site.

Behavioural tests

We identified significant measures of personality from behaviours quantified during the study. Time spent eating in a novel environment was a measure of boldness and time spent in unpreferred levels in the open field test was a measure of exploration (Table 3). These personality traits have been validated for possums and other animals in previous studies (Dammhahn and Almeling 2012; Raña 2017; Wat et al. 2020a).


Table 3.  Generalised linear mixed models for behavioural measures in brushtail possums (n = 99).
Click to zoom

The personality of hand-reared juvenile possums differed significantly from that of wild juvenile possums quantified in previous studies (Fig. 3). Hand-reared juvenile possums were significantly bolder [t(20) = 3.25, P = 0.004; Fig. 3a] and more explorative [t(19) = 4.75, P = 0.0001; Fig. 3b] than wild juvenile possums.


Fig. 3.  Differences in indices of two personality traits (a) boldness and (b) exploration, between hand-reared and wild juvenile brushtail possums. Asterisks indicate significance at P ≤ 0.01.
Click to zoom

Possums ‘buddied’ by WIRES during weaning (assigned pair number) showed increased likelihood of similar personality traits. Pair number significantly affected both boldness (F10 = 22.94, P = 0.00003) and exploration (F10 = 9.49, P = 0.001).

Human habituation

Human habituation was significantly affected by time in care (WR22056_IE1.gif = 9.61, P = 0.008). Possums that were in care for longer periods of time had higher human habituation scores (Fig. 4). Sex (WR22056_IE2.gif = 1.40, P = 0.50) and personality, boldness (WR22056_IE3.gif = 2.32, P = 0.31) and exploration (WR22056_IE4.gif = 1.74, P = 0.42), did not significantly affect the level of human habituation.


Fig. 4.  Relationship between time in care and level of human habituation for juvenile hand-reared brushtail possums.
Click to zoom

Survival

Eight possums (five males and three females) survived (40%), nine possums (four males and five females) did not survive (45%) and three possums (one male and two females) had unknown fate (15%). Successful possums survived at least 2 weeks in the wild before collars came off (average of 23.9 ± 10.2 days in the study for successful possums). Failed releases included possum individuals that had to return into care because they were struggling in the wild (n = 4; 20%) or died (n = 5; 25%) before the end of the study (average of 10.1 ± 9.6 days in the study for failed possums). Possums with unknown fate either experienced collar failure or the collars came off early in the study period (average 5.6 ± 2.1 days in the study for unknown possums).

Of the four possums returned into care, two required veterinary treatment due to health issues including dermatitis and injuries. The remaining two possums returned to WIRES care due to repeated engagement with humans and incapacity to self-sufficiently forage.

The remains of dead possums were retrieved within 48 h to ensure minimal decomposition. In three cases, the head and a limb remained still attached to the collar, and in two other instances the remains were buried in the ground or found near fox den sites and the collars had visible bite marks and scratches. Fox (Vulpes vulpes) predation inherently involves removing the head of prey, eating gut content and burying remains in soft soil (Augee et al. 1996). Therefore, the five dead possums were recorded as fox kills.

Personality and human habituation significantly affected survival. More exploratory (WR22056_IE5.gif = 6.46, P = 0.011, Fig. 5) and less human-habituated possums (WR22056_IE6.gif = 10.76, P = 0.005, Fig. 6) survived more after release. Boldness (WR22056_IE7.gif = 0.08, P = 0.780), release type (WR22056_IE8.gif = 0.53, P = 0.467), release environment (WR22056_IE9.gif = 0.004, P = 0.950) and sex (WR22056_IE10.gif = 0.56, P = 0.455) did not affect survival.


Fig. 5.  Mean ± s.e. of failed and successful releases in relation to the personality trait, exploration, of hand-raised brushtail possums. Asterisk indicates significance at P ≤ 0.05.
Click to zoom


Fig. 6.  Survival of brushtail possums in relation to their level of human habituation. Sample size for each group is indicated in bold.
Click to zoom


Discussion

Our study explored intrinsic (those related to the characteristics of individuals) and extrinsic factors (those related to the characteristic of the environment) that may affect post-release survival of hand-raised possums. Our findings suggest that intrinsic behavioural factors such as individual personality and level of human habituation are of key importance to predict survival after rehabilitation.

Effect of intrinsic factors on survival

Possums that were more exploratory had a significantly better chance of survival in the wild. This pattern is similar to that found for survival of great tits (Dingemanse et al. 2004). More exploratory possums may be more efficient in finding resources, such as food and refuge; in wild adult possums, more exploratory individuals have greater diet diversity (Herath et al. 2021), and the exploration trait affects home range and core area characteristics (Wat et al. 2020b).

Survival of individual possums also depended on their level of human habituation. The majority (n = 9) of habituated possums failed in the wild (i.e. had to be taken back into care or were killed by predators), whereas all possums with a low human-habituation score (n = 4) were successful releases (i.e. survived until the end of the study). Possums rescued at lower body weights required longer time in care, and this in turn affected the level of human habituation of the individuals. WIRES will rear pouch young possums with a body mass of >120 g, with ears detached from the body and eyes open (WIRES 2017). However, we suggest these guidelines should be reconsidered, unless hand-rearing strategies can be adapted to minimise the extent of human habituation. Highly habituated possums frequently engaged with humans during monitoring, suggesting some sort of dependence on human support, a trait not desirable in wild animals. Fearfulness towards humans (measured as avoidance and/or stress related behaviours) before release in the wild was the strongest predictor of survival after translocation for adult possums (May et al. 2016). Although human habituation is commonly noted as a factor to consider during the rehabilitation of native wildlife (Tribe and Brown 2000), only a few studies have assessed its impact on release success (Elsbeth McPhee 2004). Based on our findings, time spent in care should be minimised and/or behaviours linked to human habituation should be monitored and preferably prevented during the rehabilitation process.

We found that sex had no effect on the short-term survival of hand-reared juvenile possums, which is consistent with observations for Tasmanian devils [Sarcophilus harrisii; Sinn et al (2014)]. However, male and female juvenile possums have different dispersal patterns (Bannister et al. 2019), so 40 days may be insufficient to detect these differences and their effect on survival. Sex can affect survival of released individuals of other species (e.g. little penguins in Goldsworthy et al. 2000), and should therefore always be considered. For possums, other factors appear to be more influential in predicting success in the wild.

Effect of extrinsic factors on survival

Survival in the wild did not depend upon the release method used (as in Bannister et al. 2020). It appears that the best release method varies among species, with Swift foxes (Sasmal et al. 2015), Western burrowing owls (Athene cunicularia hypugaea) and dormice (Muscardinus avellanarius) (Bright and Morris 1994), benefiting from a gradual release method (e.g. food and shelter provided). Other species, (such as passerine birds, Richardson et al. 2015) prefer a hard release (e.g. immediate independence and no support), and some, such as eastern barred bandicoots (Perameles gunnii) (de Milliano et al. 2016), hare-wallabies (Lagorchestes hirsutus and Lagostrophus fasciatus) (Hardman and Moro 2006), western quolls (Dasyurus geoffroii) (Jensen et al. 2021), and eastern grey kangaroos (Macropus giganteus) (Campbell and Croft 2001), show no clear advantage with either method. There is currently no standardised set of techniques for the release of rehabilitated possums, so we suggest that either method can be used with no detrimental consequences.

A quarter (n = 5) of our possums were killed by foxes shortly after release. The release sites used in our study were selected by wildlife rehabilitators. To the best of our knowledge, information on the presence or absence of predators is rarely incorporated when selecting potential release sites. In the future, wildlife rehabilitators could use local databases (e.g. www.feralscan.org.au) to assess where predators are present (West 2017). The use of these types of databases would allow an assessment of the likelihood of encountering a feral predator at a selected release site and could help determining potential release locations (in terms of predator absence). Discussions with local land managers at various levels of government may also prove helpful in identifying sites where some level of fox control has been applied. Our study showed that an appropriate release site is most likely an area where feral predators are controlled. Because there appears to be an acclimation period after release (3–4 weeks) during which animals are most vulnerable to predation (Hamilton et al. 2010; Bannister et al. 2020), at sites where predators are controlled, possums would have more time to acclimate to natural conditions, without the additional pressure of dealing with risk of predation.

Another aspect of rehabilitation programmes that can determine survival is habitat quality at the release site (Cheyne 2006; Moorhouse et al. 2009). We used release environment (i.e. urban or rural) as a proxy for habitat quality, using percentage tree cover and level of urban development to classify release sites. We found no significant effect of release environment on survival for possums. However, our sample size was limited. More exploratory possums living on the boundary of a National Park and urban areas are more likely to use urban environments than less exploratory ones (Wat et al. 2020b). Thus, although it may seem preferable to release animals at rural sites that may provide more natural habitat rich of food resources and refuge (Fischer and Lindenmayer 2000; Cheyne 2006; Moorhouse et al. 2009), the effect of personality on space use by individuals should also be considered in future rehabilitation projects.

Effect of captivity on personality

Hand-reared possums had significantly different personality traits than wild individuals. Many studies have shown that personality traits can differ in captivity due to the change in selection pressures on behaviour (Price 1999; Elsbeth McPhee 2004; O’Regan and Kitchener 2005). In our study, captive juvenile possums were considerably more exploratory and bolder than wild juvenile possums in the open-field test. Hand-reared animals may lose some of the natural inhibition that juvenile possums display in the wild, and this is likely to increase their vulnerability to predators. Captive-bred grey partridges (Perdix perdix) show poor vigilance behaviour compared with wild individuals and, as a result, suffer high predation rates post-release (Rantanen et al. 2010).

The personality of the possums was also influenced by the buddy system, which is commonly used by wildlife rehabilitators. Possums that were ‘buddied’ and grew up together had similar personalities. The buddy system is used to limit imprinting on humans and encourage social learning from conspecifics (Dindo et al. 2011; Schwartz et al. 2016), but our results show it can also affect the development of personality traits of individuals, and suggest that it might limit behavioural variability in hand-reared captive possums. The particular selection pressures (i.e. predator absence, food quality or housing facilities) driving this pattern are currently unknown. but could be explored in future studies. The difference in selection pressures between wild and captive settings (McPhee and Carlstead 2010), and the consequent behavioural differences of individuals experiencing these conditions, could contribute to the high failure rate of rehabilitation and reintroduction programmes (Fischer and Lindenmayer 2000; Guy et al. 2013).

Management implications

Our study highlights the detrimental impact of introduced predators on hand-reared juvenile possums, with the European red fox being responsible for all possum mortalities (most occurring within 3 days post-release). Wild possums regularly have to deal with predation risk (Mella et al. 2014), whereas hand-reared or captive bred animals often lack predator detection and avoidance behaviours (Moseby et al. 2012). The exclusion of foxes was a key factor in the successful reintroduction of brushtail possums in Western Australia (Short and Hide 2014) and in the Flinders Ranges in South Australia (Moseby et al. 2020). The presence of predators at the release site appears to be a critical factor responsible for the failure of many release programmes (Fischer and Lindenmayer 2000; Moseby et al. 2011; Short 2016). Our failed release rate (45%) is comparable to the mortality rate reported in a translocation study on possums, where 42% of individuals died due to predation by introduced predators soon after release (Pietsch 1995). This indicates that the risk of predation is particularly high after release to a new environment.

During rehabilitation, possums are housed in aviaries in suburban settings with limited exposure to natural stimuli, and are often raised in the presence of pets, such as cats and dogs, which may cause habituation to potential predators, and a lack of danger perception in juvenile hand-reared possums. Possums are arboreal animals and although they can spend time on the ground to feed and to move between foraging patches, they generally prefer the safety of trees (MacLennan 1984). However, rehabilitated arboreal animals often spend more time on the ground than their wild counterparts (Basalamah et al. 2018) and this may lower survival prospects. Rehabilitated possums in our study might have spent more time on the ground (as in Bannister et al. 2018) than wild juvenile possums, and thus might have been more vulnerable to predation. Rearing guidelines should be developed to encourage wariness in hand-raised animals (Rantanen et al. 2010).

The threat of predation of hand-reared possums could potentially be minimised using a number of approaches. The potential link between habitation to humans and predation should be further explored to ascertain whether minimising habituation improves release survival. Another potential management technique is predator avoidance training before release. This usually involves exposing animals to predator stimuli (e.g. predator model, tactile, audio and/or olfactory stimuli) associated with a negative feedback (Moseby et al. 2012). Predator avoidance training can improve success rates of animals post-release (Van Heezik et al. 1999; West et al. 2018) and may be essential for hand-reared individuals lacking any previous predation risk experience. Simple and inexpensive forms of anti-predator training should be explored as a viable option for rehabilitation programmes. Limiting association with pets while in care is also fundamental.

During our study, we had to intervene and rescue four possums that were placed back into care. These individuals showed inappropriate behavioural responses to life in the wild, were habituated to humans, lacked independent foraging skills and would have most likely died without intervention. Reducing the extent of habituation would hopefully reduce the likelihood of this occurring in the future. If reduced habituation alone does not sufficiently reduce the incidence of inappropriate behavioural responses post release, additional approaches, such as pre-release locomotor and foraging training (Kleiman 1989), could potentially improve post-release success rates. Previous research has shown that facilitating and eliciting natural behaviours in captivity increases foraging and locomotion skills post-release in the wild (Stoinski and Beck 2004; Schwartz et al. 2016), and potentially improves release success (Humle et al. 2011). Because not all individuals that undergo rehabilitation seem to be suited for release, we urge the rehabilitation sector to consider the ethical consequences and implications of releasing animals that have a poor chance of post-release survival.

Studies also suggest that rehabilitation enrichment, in the form of facility modification, can have an impact on survival rates in the wild (Kelly et al. 2008). For example, pipistrelle bats (Pipistrellus pipistrellus) have increased survival rates when housed in large flight cages compared with wildlife rehabilitators’ homes (Kelly et al. 2008). For arboreal marsupials such as possums, locomotion and climbing skills are a requirement for foraging and predator avoidance in the wild. Ensuring appropriate rehabilitation aviaries large enough to encourage natural climbing and foraging behaviours may therefore be fundamental for their success post-release.

General conclusions and future directions

We found that intrinsic factors (those specifically related to the characteristics of the individuals) were more important in determining success of hand-reared possums than extrinsic factors (those related to the release method or site). Naturally, there could be a number of other factors that may affect post-release success that could not be explored here due to our small sample size. By identifying some of the factors that contribute to the success of release programmes, our study aimed to help inform management strategies for rehabilitation of wildlife. These can be implemented in the form of behavioural and management modifications (e.g. anti-predator and foraging training), and efficient selection of release method and release site. Although our results have possible applications for a wide range of wildlife, it is important to note that specific strategies may need to be tailored depending on the species considered (Moseby et al. 2014).

Our study also reiterates the importance of understanding animal personality in rehabilitation programmes, including how this can differ between hand-reared and wild populations. Future studies may provide more information on which aspects of personality are being altered by captivity and which are the most damaging when individuals are placed into a wild context. Our study has contributed to identifying the negative effects of time in care and human habituation on survival of wildlife.

Post-release monitoring of individuals can help us to determine factors impacting release success and understand important ecological interactions (e.g. invasive predator effects) (Moseby et al. 2021). It is therefore important that rehabilitation programmes invest in post-release monitoring as much as possible. Our study was short-term, so we were only able to monitor survival of a small number of individuals for a maximum of 40 days. Ideally, future studies should monitor released individuals for longer to determine long-term (e.g. 2 years) survival, dispersal/establishment and reproductive viability of released animals. Comparisons with wild conspecifics would also help to establish the extent to which rates and causes of mortality vary between hand-reared and wild animals.

The results of our study can be used to ensure that the limited funds available to wildlife rehabilitation organisations are spent in the most effective way for release programmes, and that the substantial time investment needed to foster wildlife to the release stage translates to the best chance of survival for the animals. Rehabilitation programmes are an important part of the welfare of native wildlife. With further scientific support, rehabilitation programmes could also positively contribute to the conservation of threatened species.


Data availability

The data used to generate the results in the paper will be shared on the Sydney eScholarship Repository https://ses.library.usyd.edu.au.


Conflicts of interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.


Declaration of funding

This research was funded by the University of Sydney NSW Industry and Community Seed Fund (#CT19595, awarded to CAH, CM and VM, in collaboration with the NSW Wildlife Information, Rescue and Education Service Inc.).



Acknowledgements

We thank the WIRES volunteers who provided possums for the study – in particular, Laura Turner, Kris Spasojevic and Tracey Russell for their interest in the project and coordination of possums and release sites. We also thank Lena Aahlby, Peter Maresch and David George for allowing us to track possums on their properties. Thanks to Mark Porter and the staff at Concord Golf Club. Special thanks to Angela Raña, Dr. Katie Wat and Dr. Adrian Rus for help with radiotracking and animal handling.


References

Ario, A, Kartono, A, Prasetyo, LB, and Supriatna, J (2018). Post-release adaptation of Javan gibbon (Hylobates moloch) in Mount Malabar Protected Forest, West Java, Indonesia. Biodiversitas Journal of Biological Diversity 19, 1482–1491.
Post-release adaptation of Javan gibbon (Hylobates moloch) in Mount Malabar Protected Forest, West Java, Indonesia.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Augee, ML, Smith, B, and Rose, S (1996). Survival of wild and hand-reared ringtail possums (Pseudocheirus peregrinus) in bushland near Sydney. Wildlife Research 23, 99–108.
Survival of wild and hand-reared ringtail possums (Pseudocheirus peregrinus) in bushland near Sydney.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Bannister, H, Brandle, R, and Moseby, K (2018). Antipredator behaviour of a native marsupial is relaxed when mammalian predators are excluded. Wildlife Research 45, 726–736.
Antipredator behaviour of a native marsupial is relaxed when mammalian predators are excluded.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Bannister, HL, Hodgens, P, and Moseby, KE (2019). Offspring sex and maternal effects influence the development and natal dispersal of an arboreal marsupial. Journal of Mammalogy 100, 423–434.
Offspring sex and maternal effects influence the development and natal dispersal of an arboreal marsupial.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Bannister, HL, Brandle, R, Delean, S, Paton, DC, and Moseby, KE (2020). Supportive release techniques provide no reintroduction benefit when efficacy and uptake is low. Oryx 54, 206–214.
Supportive release techniques provide no reintroduction benefit when efficacy and uptake is low.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Basalamah, F, Atmoko, SSU, Perwitasari-Farajallah, D, Qayim, I, Sihite, J, van Noordwijk, M, Willems, E, and van Schaik, CP (2018). Monitoring orangutan reintroduction: results of activity budgets, diets, vertical use and associations during the first year post-release in Kehje Sewen Forest, East Kalimantan, Indonesia. Biodiversitas Journal of Biological Diversity 19, 639–650.
Monitoring orangutan reintroduction: results of activity budgets, diets, vertical use and associations during the first year post-release in Kehje Sewen Forest, East Kalimantan, Indonesia.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Blumstein DT, Daniel JC (2007) ‘Quantifying Behaviour the JWatcher Way.’ (Sinauer Associates: Sunderland, MA, USA)

Bremner-Harrison, S, Prodohl, PA, and Elwood, RW (2004). Behavioural trait assessment as a release criterion: boldness predicts early death in a reintroduction programme of captive-bred swift fox (Vulpes velox. Animal Conservation 7, 313–320.
Behavioural trait assessment as a release criterion: boldness predicts early death in a reintroduction programme of captive-bred swift fox (Vulpes velox.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Bright, PW, and Morris, PA (1994). Animal translocation for conservation: performance of dormice in relation to release methods, origin and season. Journal of Applied Ecology 31, 699–708.
Animal translocation for conservation: performance of dormice in relation to release methods, origin and season.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Campbell L, Croft DB (2001) Comparison of hard and soft release of hand-reared eastern grey kangaroos. In ‘Veterinary Conservation Biology, Wildlife Health and Management in Australasia. Proceedings of International Joint Conference’. (Eds A Martin, L Vogelnest) pp. 173–180. (Taronga Zoo: Sydney, NSW, Australia)

Cawthen, L, and Munks, S (2011). The design and testing of linen thread weak-links in brushtail possum radio-collars. Australian Mammalogy 33, 33–35.
The design and testing of linen thread weak-links in brushtail possum radio-collars.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Cheyne, SM (2006). Wildlife reintroduction: considerations of habitat quality at the release site. BMC Ecology 6, 5.
Wildlife reintroduction: considerations of habitat quality at the release site.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Cope, H, McArthur, C, Dickman, C, Newsome, T, Gray, R, and Herbert, C (2022a). A systematic review of factors affecting wildlife survival during rehabilitation and release. PLoS ONE 17, e0265514.
A systematic review of factors affecting wildlife survival during rehabilitation and release.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Cope, HR, Keeley, T, Keong, J, Smith, D, Silva, FRO, McArthur, C, Webster, KN, Mella, VSA, and Herbert, CA (2022b). Validation of an enzyme immunoassay to measure faecal glucocorticoid metabolites in common brushtail possums (Trichosurus vulpecula) to evaluate responses to rehabilitation. Animals 12, 1627.
Validation of an enzyme immunoassay to measure faecal glucocorticoid metabolites in common brushtail possums (Trichosurus vulpecula) to evaluate responses to rehabilitation.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Dammhahn, M, and Almeling, L (2012). Is risk taking during foraging a personality trait? A field test for cross-context consistency in boldness. Animal Behaviour 84, 1131–1139.
Is risk taking during foraging a personality trait? A field test for cross-context consistency in boldness.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

de Milliano, J, Di Stefano, J, Courtney, P, Temple-Smith, P, and Coulson, G (2016). Soft-release versus hard-release for reintroduction of an endangered species: an experimental comparison using eastern barred bandicoots (Perameles gunnii. Wildlife Research 43, 1–12.
Soft-release versus hard-release for reintroduction of an endangered species: an experimental comparison using eastern barred bandicoots (Perameles gunnii.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Dindo, M, Stoinski, T, and Whiten, A (2011). Observational learning in orangutan cultural transmission chains. Biology Letters 7, 181–183.
Observational learning in orangutan cultural transmission chains.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Dingemanse, NJ, Both, C, Drent, PJ, and Tinbergen, JM (2004). Fitness consequences of avian personalities in a fluctuating environment. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences 271, 847–852.
Fitness consequences of avian personalities in a fluctuating environment.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Eymann, J, Herbert, CA, and Cooper, DW (2006). Management issues of urban common brushtail possums Trichosurus vulpecula: a loved or hated neighbour. Australian Mammalogy 28, 153–171.
Management issues of urban common brushtail possums Trichosurus vulpecula: a loved or hated neighbour.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Fischer, J, and Lindenmayer, DB (2000). An assessment of the published results of animal relocations. Biological Conservation 96, 1–11.
An assessment of the published results of animal relocations.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Goldsworthy, SD, Gales, RP, Giese, M, and Brothers, N (2000). Effects of the Iron Baron oil spill on little penguins (Eudyptula minor). I. Estimates of mortality. Wildlife Research 27, 559–571.
Effects of the Iron Baron oil spill on little penguins (Eudyptula minor). I. Estimates of mortality.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Guy, AJ, Curnoe, D, and Banks, PB (2013). A survey of current mammal rehabilitation and release practices. Biodiversity and Conservation 22, 825–837.
A survey of current mammal rehabilitation and release practices.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Hamilton, LP, Kelly, PA, Williams, DF, Kelt, DA, and Wittmer, HU (2010). Factors associated with survival of reintroduced riparian brush rabbits in California. Biological Conservation 143, 999–1007.
Factors associated with survival of reintroduced riparian brush rabbits in California.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Hardman, B, and Moro, D (2006). Optimising reintroduction success by delayed dispersal: is the release protocol important for hare-wallabies? Biological Conservation 128, 403–411.
Optimising reintroduction success by delayed dispersal: is the release protocol important for hare-wallabies?Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Helgen KM, Jackson SM (2015) Family Phalangeridae (cuscuses, brush-tailed possums and scaly-tailed possums). In ‘Handbook of the Mammals of the World’. (Eds DE Wilson, RA Mittermeier) pp. 456–497. (Lynxs Edicions: Barcelona, Spain.)

Herath, APHM, Wat, KKY, Banks, PB, and McArthur, C (2021). Animal personality drives individual dietary specialisation across multiple dimensions in a mammalian herbivore. Functional Ecology 35, 2253–2265.
Animal personality drives individual dietary specialisation across multiple dimensions in a mammalian herbivore.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Humle, T, Colin, C, Laurans, M, and Raballand, E (2011). Group release of sanctuary chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) in the Haut Niger National Park, Guinea, West Africa: ranging patterns and lessons so far. International Journal of Primatology 32, 456–473.
Group release of sanctuary chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) in the Haut Niger National Park, Guinea, West Africa: ranging patterns and lessons so far.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Jensen, MA, Paton, DC, and Moseby, KE (2021). Delayed release improves site fidelity but has little effect on survival or breeding success of reintroduced western quolls (Dasyurus geoffroii. Austral Ecology 46, 1298–1310.
Delayed release improves site fidelity but has little effect on survival or breeding success of reintroduced western quolls (Dasyurus geoffroii.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Kelly, A, Goodwin, S, Grogan, A, and Mathews, F (2008). Post-release survival of hand-reared pipistrelle bats (Pipistrellus spp. Animal Welfare 17, 375–382.

Kerle JA (2001) ‘Possums: the Brushtails, the Ringtails and Greater Glider.’ (University of New South Wales Press: Sydney, NSW, Australia)

Kerle, JA, Foulkes, JN, Kimber, RG, and Papenfus, D (1992). The decline of the brushtail possum, Trichosurus vulpecula (Kerr 1798), in arid Australia. The Rangeland Journal 14, 107–127.
The decline of the brushtail possum, Trichosurus vulpecula (Kerr 1798), in arid Australia.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Kleiman, DG (1989). Reintroduction of captive mammals for conservation. Bioscience 39, 152–161.
Reintroduction of captive mammals for conservation.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

MacLennan DG (1984) The feeding behaviour and activity patterns of the brushtail possum, Trichosurus vulpecula, in an open eucaplypt woodland in Southeast Queensland. In ‘Possums and Gliders’. (Eds A Smith, ID Hume) pp. 155–161. (Australian Mammal Society: Sydney, NSW, Australia)

Marzluff, JM, and Ewing, K (2001). Restoration of fragmented landscapes for the conservation of birds: a general framework and specific recommendations for urbanizing landscapes. Restoration Ecology 9, 280–292.
Restoration of fragmented landscapes for the conservation of birds: a general framework and specific recommendations for urbanizing landscapes.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

May, TM, Page, MJ, and Fleming, PA (2016). Predicting survivors: animal temperament and translocation. Behavioral Ecology 27, 969–977.
Predicting survivors: animal temperament and translocation.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

McDougall, PT, Réale, D, Sol, D, and Reader, SM (2006). Wildlife conservation and animal temperament: causes and consequences of evolutionary change for captive, reintroduced, and wild populations. Animal Conservation 9, 39–48.
Wildlife conservation and animal temperament: causes and consequences of evolutionary change for captive, reintroduced, and wild populations.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

McPhee, ME (2004). Generations in captivity increases behavioral variance: considerations for captive breeding and reintroduction programs. Biological Conservation 115, 71–77.
Generations in captivity increases behavioral variance: considerations for captive breeding and reintroduction programs.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

McPhee E, Carlstead K (2010) Effects of captivity on the behavior of wild mammals. In ‘Wild Mammals in Captivity’. 2nd edn. (Eds DG Kleiman, K Thompson, M Allen) pp. 303–313. (University of Chicago Press: Chicago, IL, USA)

Mella, VSA, Banks, PB, and McArthur, C (2014). Negotiating multiple cues of predation risk in a landscape of fear: what scares free-ranging brushtail possums? Journal of Zoology 294, 22–30.
Negotiating multiple cues of predation risk in a landscape of fear: what scares free-ranging brushtail possums?Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Mella, VSA, Krucler, J, Sunderasan, L, Hawkins, J, Herath, APHM, Johnstone, KC, Troxell-Smith, SM, Banks, PB, and McArthur, C (2016). Effective field-based methods to quantify personality in brushtail possums (Trichosurus vulpecula. Wildlife Research 43, 332–340.
Effective field-based methods to quantify personality in brushtail possums (Trichosurus vulpecula.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Moorhouse, TP, Gelling, M, and Macdonald, DW (2009). Effects of habitat quality upon reintroduction success in water voles: evidence from a replicated experiment. Biological Conservation 142, 53–60.
Effects of habitat quality upon reintroduction success in water voles: evidence from a replicated experiment.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Moseby, KE, Read, JL, Paton, DC, Copley, P, Hill, BM, and Crisp, HA (2011). Predation determines the outcome of 10 reintroduction attempts in arid South Australia. Biological Conservation 144, 2863–2872.
Predation determines the outcome of 10 reintroduction attempts in arid South Australia.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Moseby, KE, Cameron, A, and Crisp, HA (2012). Can predator avoidance training improve reintroduction outcomes for the greater bilby in arid Australia? Animal Behaviour 83, 1011–1021.
Can predator avoidance training improve reintroduction outcomes for the greater bilby in arid Australia?Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Moseby, KE, Hill, BM, and Lavery, TH (2014). Tailoring release protocols to individual species and sites: one size does not fit all. PLoS ONE 9, e99753.
Tailoring release protocols to individual species and sites: one size does not fit all.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Moseby, KE, Brandle, R, Hodgens, P, and Bannister, HL (2020). Can reintroductions to degraded habitat succeed? A test using the common brushtail possum. Austral Ecology 45, 675–690.
Can reintroductions to degraded habitat succeed? A test using the common brushtail possum.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Moseby, KE, Hodgens, P, Peacock, D, Mooney, P, Brandle, R, Lynch, C, West, R, Young, CM, Bannister, H, Copley, P, and Jensen, MA (2021). Intensive monitoring, the key to identifying cat predation as a major threat to native carnivore (Dasyurus geoffroii) reintroduction. Biodiversity and Conservation 30, 1547–1571.
Intensive monitoring, the key to identifying cat predation as a major threat to native carnivore (Dasyurus geoffroii) reintroduction.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

OEH (2011) ‘Code of Practice for Injured, Sick and Orphaned Protected Fauna.’ (NSW Office of Environment and Heritage: Sydney, NSW, Australia)

O’Regan, HJ, and Kitchener, AC (2005). The effects of captivity on the morphology of captive, domesticated and feral mammals. Mammal Review 35, 215–230.
The effects of captivity on the morphology of captive, domesticated and feral mammals.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Parker KA, Dickens MJ, Clarke RH, Lovegrove TG (2012) The theory and practice of catching, holding, moving and releasing animals. In ‘Reintroduction Biology’. (Eds JG Ewen, DP Armstrong, KA Parker, PJ Seddon) pp. 105–137. (Wiley-Blackwell: West Sussex, UK.)

Pekin, BK, and Pijanowski, BC (2012). Global land use intensity and the endangerment status of mammal species. Diversity and Distributions 18, 909–918.
Global land use intensity and the endangerment status of mammal species.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Petelle, MB, McCoy, DE, Alejandro, V, Martin, JGA, and Blumstein, DT (2013). Development of boldness and docility in yellow-bellied marmots. Animal Behaviour 86, 1147–1154.
Development of boldness and docility in yellow-bellied marmots.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Pietsch RS (1995) The fate of common brushtail possums translocated to sclerophyll forest. In ‘Reintroduction Biology of Australasian Fauna’. (Ed. M Serena) pp. 239–246. (Surrey Beatty: Sydney, NSW, Australia)

Price, EO (1999). Behavioral development in animals undergoing domestication. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 65, 245–271.
Behavioral development in animals undergoing domestication.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

R Core Team (2020) ‘R: a language and environment for statistical computing.’ (R Foundation for Statistical Computing: Vienna, Austria)

Raña A (2017) The influences of physiological stress, personality, and behavioural mechanisms on problem solving by common brushtail possums. BSc Hons thesis, University of Sydney, NSW, Australia.

Rantanen, EM, Buner, F, Riordan, P, Sotherton, N, and Macdonald, DW (2010). Vigilance, time budgets and predation risk in reintroduced captive-bred grey partridges Perdix perdix. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 127, 43–50.
Vigilance, time budgets and predation risk in reintroduced captive-bred grey partridges Perdix perdix.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Rasmussen, SL, Kalliokoski, O, Dabelsteen, T, and Abelson, K (2021). An exploratory investigation of glucocorticoids, personality and survival rates in wild and rehabilitated hedgehogs (Erinaceus europaeus) in Denmark. BMC Ecology and Evolution 21, 96.
An exploratory investigation of glucocorticoids, personality and survival rates in wild and rehabilitated hedgehogs (Erinaceus europaeus) in Denmark.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Richardson, K, Castro, IC, Brunton, DH, and Armstrong, DP (2015). Not so soft? Delayed release reduces long-term survival in a passerine reintroduction. Oryx 49, 535–541.
Not so soft? Delayed release reduces long-term survival in a passerine reintroduction.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Rödl, T, Berger, S, Romero, LM, and Wikelski, M (2007). Tameness and stress physiology in a predator-naive island species confronted with novel predation threat. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 274, 577–582.
Tameness and stress physiology in a predator-naive island species confronted with novel predation threat.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Russell, TC, Herbert, CA, and Kohen, JL (2009). High possum mortality on urban roads: implications for the population viability of the common brushtail and the common ringtail possum. Australian Journal of Zoology 57, 391–397.
High possum mortality on urban roads: implications for the population viability of the common brushtail and the common ringtail possum.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Sasmal, I, Honness, K, Bly, K, McCaffery, M, Kunkel, K, Jenks, JA, and Phillips, M (2015). Release method evaluation for swift fox reintroduction at Bad River Ranches in South Dakota. Restoration Ecology 23, 491–498.
Release method evaluation for swift fox reintroduction at Bad River Ranches in South Dakota.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Schwartz, JW, Hopkins, ME, and Hopkins, SL (2016). Group prerelease training yields positive rehabilitation outcomes among juvenile mantled howlers (Alouatta palliata. International Journal of Primatology 37, 260–280.
Group prerelease training yields positive rehabilitation outcomes among juvenile mantled howlers (Alouatta palliata.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Short, J (2016). Predation by feral cats key to the failure of a long-term reintroduction of the western barred bandicoot (Perameles bougainville. Wildlife Research 43, 38–50.
Predation by feral cats key to the failure of a long-term reintroduction of the western barred bandicoot (Perameles bougainville.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Short, J, and Hide, A (2014). Successful reintroduction of the brushtail possum to Wadderin Sanctuary in the eastern wheatbelt of Western Australia. Australian Mammalogy 36, 229–241.
Successful reintroduction of the brushtail possum to Wadderin Sanctuary in the eastern wheatbelt of Western Australia.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Sinn, DL, Cawthen, L, Jones, SM, Pukk, C, and Jones, ME (2014). Boldness towards novelty and translocation success in captive-raised, orphaned Tasmanian devils. Zoo Biology 33, 36–48.
Boldness towards novelty and translocation success in captive-raised, orphaned Tasmanian devils.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Stoinski, TS, and Beck, BB (2004). Changes in locomotor and foraging skills in captive-born, reintroduced golden lion tamarins (Leontopithecus rosalia rosalia. American Journal of Primatology 62, 1–13.
Changes in locomotor and foraging skills in captive-born, reintroduced golden lion tamarins (Leontopithecus rosalia rosalia.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Tarsitano, E (2006). Interaction between the environment and animals in urban settings: Integrated and participatory planning. Environmental Management 38, 799–809.
Interaction between the environment and animals in urban settings: Integrated and participatory planning.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Teixeira, CP, de Azevedo, CS, Mendl, M, Cipreste, CF, and Young, RJ (2007). Revisiting translocation and reintroduction programmes: the importance of considering stress. Animal Behaviour 73, 1–13.
Revisiting translocation and reintroduction programmes: the importance of considering stress.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Tolhurst, B, Grogan, A, Hughes, H, and Scott, D (2016). Effects of temporary captivity on ranging behaviour in urban red foxes (Vulpes vulpes. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 181, 182–190.
Effects of temporary captivity on ranging behaviour in urban red foxes (Vulpes vulpes.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Tribe, A, and Brown, PR (2000). The role of wildlife rescue groups in the care and rehabilitation of Australian fauna. Human Dimensions of Wildlife 5, 69–85.
The role of wildlife rescue groups in the care and rehabilitation of Australian fauna.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

van Heezik, Y, Seddon, PJ, and Maloney, RF (1999). Helping reintroduced houbara bustards avoid predation: effective anti-predator training and the predictive value of pre-release behaviour. Animal Conservation 2, 155–163.
Helping reintroduced houbara bustards avoid predation: effective anti-predator training and the predictive value of pre-release behaviour.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Wat, KKY, Banks, PB, and McArthur, C (2020a). Linking animal personality to problem-solving performance in urban common brushtail possums. Animal Behaviour 162, 35–45.
Linking animal personality to problem-solving performance in urban common brushtail possums.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Wat, KKY, Herath, APHM, Rus, AI, Banks, PB, and Mcarthur, C (2020b). Space use by animals on the urban fringe: interactive effects of sex and personality. Behavioral Ecology 31, 330–339.
Space use by animals on the urban fringe: interactive effects of sex and personality.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

West P (2017) Feralscan. Available at https://www.feralscan.org.au/

West, R, Letnic, M, Blumstein, DT, and Moseby, KE (2018). Predator exposure improves anti-predator responses in a threatened mammal. Journal of Applied Ecology 55, 147–156.
Predator exposure improves anti-predator responses in a threatened mammal.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Winer BJ, Brown DR, Michels KM (1991) ‘Statistical Principles in Experimental Design.’ 3rd edn. (McGraw-Hill: New York, NY, USA)

WIRES (2017) ‘Rescue, Rehabilitation and Release of Possums and Gliders.’ 5th edn. (WIRES Small Mammals Team: Sydney, NSW, Australia)

WIRES (2019) Annual report 2019. NSW Wildlife Information, Rescue and Education Service Inc, Sydney, NSW, Australia.